CONTEXT:

Young children have increasing access to interactive applications (apps) at home and at school. Existing research is clear on the potential dangers of overuse of screens, but there is less clarity around the extent to which interactive apps may be helpful in supporting early learning.

OBJECTIVE:

In this systematic review, we present a narrative synthesis of studies examining whether children <6 years can learn from interactive apps.

DATA SOURCES:

The PsycInfo, PubMed, ACM Digital Library, and ERIC databases were searched.

STUDY SELECTION:

Studies were included if the study design was randomized or nonrandomized controlled (quasi-experimental), the sample mean age was <6 years, the intervention involved children playing with an interactive app, and academic, cognitive, or social-emotional skill outcomes were measured.

DATA EXTRACTION:

Of 1447 studies, 35 were included.

RESULTS:

Evidence of a learning benefit of interactive app use for early academic skills was found across multiple studies, particularly for early mathematics learning in typically developing children. Researchers did not find evidence of an intervention effect for apps aiming to improve social communication skills in children with autism spectrum disorder.

LIMITATIONS:

Risk of bias was unclear for many studies because of inadequate reporting. Studies were highly heterogenous in interventions, outcomes, and study design, making comparisons of results across studies difficult.

CONCLUSIONS:

There is emerging evidence to suggest that interactive apps may be useful and accessible tools for supporting early academic development. More research is needed to evaluate both the potential of educational apps to support early learning, and their limitations.

Touchscreen mobile devices (eg, smartphones, tablets) have become ubiquitous for young children.1  Interactive applications or “apps” considered “educational” for young children have similarly gained in popularity2  and are increasingly being integrated into early childhood classrooms as learning tools because of perceived advantages for child engagement and active learning.3  The integration of interactive app technology into children’s lives at home and school has outpaced research needed to inform comprehensive recommendations for its use. Recommendations have thus far focused on preventing overuse of screens4  rather than opportunities for maximizing learning. Research on whether young children can learn from interactive apps; the academic, cognitive, or social-emotional skill domains that may be best supported by interactive apps; and the conditions under which this learning may be maximized; is still emerging.

In 2016, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommended screen time (including touchscreens) for 2- to 5-year-olds be limited to <1 hour per day of “high-quality educational programming” and that parents coview screens with children.5  Given the scarcity of studies on interactive screen media, the AAP statement largely referenced studies from the more-extensive literature on television, in which researchers documented the detrimental effects of overconsumption of television on sleep,6  obesity,7  and cognitive and language development8  but also showed that high-quality and developmentally appropriate programming (eg, Sesame Street)9  can support vocabulary, math, and social-emotional skills in children >30 months.10,11 

Theories on learning suggest children learn best when activities promote active “minds-on” learning, are appropriately scaffolded, and are engaging.2  Additionally, research shows that young children benefit from repeated and varied practice of foundational skills.12,13  Interactive apps have potential to support active engagement with learning material through embedding learning concepts into gamelike activities, scaffolding children’s learning through adaptive learning technology, providing feedback and rewards through gameplay, and promoting repeated practice of important foundational skills.2,14  Additionally, the ubiquity of touchscreen devices could facilitate more equitable and convenient access to educational materials in the home, in contrast to other types of technology (eg, computers), which still show marked income gaps in access.1  Research on whether interactive educational touchscreen media can support early learning is therefore essential for updating our understanding of “high-quality educational programming” for the new digital age.

Studies in diverse fields, including psychology, education, and computer science, have begun to investigate children’s learning from apps, with most studies published in the past 3 years. The findings of these studies have not yet been well synthesized. In existing review articles, authors have provided commentaries on children’s use of mobile technology, rather than systematic reviews of the literature.15,16  In one recent meta-analysis, authors reported a significant positive effect across studies of young children’s learning from touchscreen devices.17  However, there was significant clinical and methodological heterogeneity in the included studies, which encompassed varied types of interventions (eg, electronic books, interactive game apps), outcomes targeted, comparison groups, and study designs (randomized and nonrandomized, controlled and noncontrolled). Statistical heterogeneity in this study was high (I2 >90%), limiting interpretability of a meta-analytic effect size. Instead, a narrative synthesis of results is the recommended approach when studies are highly variable in interventions, outcomes, study design, and risk of bias.18,19 

Thus, in this study, we aimed to systematically review the rapidly expanding but heterogeneous body of evidence for young children’s learning from touchscreen interactive apps. Given the popularity of interactive educational apps for young children; the importance of early learning and school readiness for later academic, behavioral, and emotional functioning20,21 ; and the existing questions about whether young children can apply information learned on a screen to real life15 ; we focused on children younger than 6 years in this review. The specific research questions were as follows:

  1. Is there evidence that children <6 years can learn from using interactive touchscreen apps?

  2. What content subjects or skills are particularly well suited to touchscreen app learning?

  3. How does learning from touchscreen interactive app games compare with children’s learning in other instructional contexts, including in-person instruction or educational video?

An electronic database search was conducted in 4 databases popular to the fields of psychology, pediatrics, education, and computer science (PsycINFO, PubMed, ERIC, and ACM Digital Library) by using the following Boolean search phrase: (tablets OR iPads OR touchscreen OR apps OR interactive media) AND (learning OR literacy OR mathematics OR skills OR development OR functioning OR outcomes OR achievement) AND (toddlers OR young children OR preschoolers). Articles published before January 1, 2008 (the year Apple and Android app stores opened), and written in languages other than English were excluded. The database searches were conducted on January 22, 2019. Additionally, the reference sections of included articles and relevant review articles were examined to identify any other studies meeting inclusion criteria (n = 2).

Study inclusion and exclusion criteria are presented in Table 1. The search initially identified 1447 unique records. After initial screening, 115 records were subject to full text review by 2 independent coders from a group of 4 (authors S.F.G., B.H.H., M.B.H., P.H.). Disagreements were discussed, and consensus was reached. Randolph’s κ22,23  was 0.84, indicating excellent interrater reliability. Thirty-four articles, describing 35 studies, were included (see Fig 1 for Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses diagram).

The following information was extracted from each study: design, sample size, location of study, language of participants, length of intervention, type of control group, name of app(s), study context, targeted subject(s) of intervention, outcome measures, and results. Given the number of articles (n = 12) for which there was not sufficient information to calculate an effect size and the clinical, methodological, and statistical heterogeneity of the included studies in terms of design, outcomes measured, and comparison group used, formal meta-analytic strategies were not used to summarize overall effects.18,19  Instead, we presented a narrative synthesis of the included studies according to the targeted subject of the intervention.19  Additionally, to provide context as to clinical significance of study findings, where possible, we presented ranges of effect sizes (using Cohen’s d) representing post-test differences between the app intervention and control groups for the randomized studies.

Because most included studies had a randomized design, risk of bias was assessed with the Cochrane’s risk of bias tool for randomized trials24  that was adapted to include only categories relevant to psychological and educational research. Studies were rated in the following categories: randomization (selection bias), blinding of outcome assessment (detection bias), missing data (attrition bias), and selective reporting (reporting bias). Quasi-experimental studies, for which assignment to group was not random (eg, administrative convenience; n = 5), were also evaluated by using this tool to capture risk of bias due to nonrandomized assignment in the randomization category. Ratings of “high,” “low,” or “unclear” risk were assigned for each domain. Each study was coded for risk of bias independently by 2 coders from a group of 4 (coders stated above). Kappa values were 0.91, 0.61, 0.51, and 0.96 for randomization, missing data, outcome assessment, and selective reporting, respectively, indicating moderate to excellent agreement.25  All disagreements were discussed, and a consensus was reached between coders.

The total number of participants across studies was 4639. Eighteen studies were conducted in the United States, 5 in Australia, 4 in the United Kingdom, 2 in Canada, and 1 in each of the United Arab Emirates, Italy, Greece, Croatia, the Netherlands, and Germany. The primary language of most of the samples (n = 30) was English (for studies conducted in the United States, Australia, the United Kingdom, Canada, and the United Arab Emirates). The 5 remaining studies included samples in which the primary language spoken was not English (Italian, Greek, Croatian, Dutch, and German, respectively); in each case, the interventions and assessments were delivered in the primary language of the sample. Samples included typically developing children, except for the studies in which researchers examined social communication (n = 3), which all consisted of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). In 12 studies, researchers used apps developed by the study authors (noted in Table 2); in 4 of these 12 studies, the app was commercially available, suggesting potential financial profit for the author.2629  In the remaining studies, researchers used existing apps developed by nonrelated companies.

Twenty-nine studies had randomized designs, including 18 randomized controlled trials (RCTs) with app interventions delivered over an extended period (ranging from 5 days to 1 academic year) and 11 laboratory-based experimental studies, which included interventions (ie, learning phases) delivered over a short time span (ranging from 5 to 20 minutes). For simplicity, studies with randomized designs and interventions delivered over an extended time are referred to as RCTs. Laboratory-based experiments (all of which also have randomized designs but a short-term intervention phase) are referred to as experimental studies. The remaining 6 studies included in this review were quasi-experimental (ie, nonrandomized controlled) trials. Comparison groups varied across studies, including in-person instruction (eg, classroom-based, in-person, using manipulatives), video instruction, or other non–app-based instruction (eg, pen and paper practice); attention controls receiving apps not targeting the identified outcome; or groups receiving no treatment. Five studies included interventions delivered at home, 18 included interventions delivered in the school, and 12 were delivered in a laboratory. The studies had a variety of outcomes in targeted educational domains, including mathematics (n = 15), language arts (LA) (n = 11), executive functioning (n = 5), health and science knowledge (n = 3), and social communication (n = 3). One study included targeted outcomes in both mathematics and science,62  and another included math and LA outcomes.60  Each of the studies examined potential learning or skill benefits of interactive apps. Although not specifically excluded from the search strategy, no identified study was designed to examine harmful effects of interactive app use on functioning. See Table 2 for a summary of studies.

Overall, 19 of the 35 studies (54%) were at high risk of bias in at least 1 domain. High risk of bias ratings were given for nonrandom assignment to groups, substantial missing data and data not missing at random (eg, imbalances in missing data between groups), and lack of blinding of outcome assessors. Additionally, risk of bias was judged to be unclear due to inadequate reporting of methods in at least 2 domains for 19 of the 35 studies. Only 3 studies had low risk of bias across the randomization, missing data, and outcome assessment domains, and no studies were given low risk ratings across all 4 domains. The results of the studies should be interpreted with caution given the literature lacks rigorously designed RCTs.

Early Math Learning

In 12 randomized studies (9 RCTs) and 3 quasi-experimental studies, researchers examined children’s math outcomes after using math interactive apps. In 11 of the 15 studies, researchers examined school-based interventions; a home-based intervention was examined in 1; and 3 were laboratory-based experiments. Outcomes included measures of early math skills (including number recognition, number naming, and basic addition and subtraction). School and home interventions ranged from 5 days to 1 academic year. Overall, 7 of the 12 randomized studies (including 1 of the 3 laboratory-based experiments) and all 3 quasi-experimental studies showed better outcomes in the math app intervention group compared with the control group used.

Specifically, of the 9 studies (6 RCTs) that compared a math app intervention to in-person classroom instruction, 7 (4 RCTs) reported significantly greater math learning gains on early math measures (n = 8) and a number-writing task (n = 1) for groups using interactive math apps,26,27,36,38,39,41,60  whereas the remaining 2 RCTs reported statistically equivalent gains between the groups.33,35  Of the 4 studies (3 RCTs) in which researchers compared a math app intervention to control groups using apps not targeting math (ie, attention controls) or not receiving treatment, 3 reported better outcomes on early math measures for app intervention groups compared with controls,28,31,40  and 1 reported no significant differences between groups.32  Effect sizes, when reported (n = 9, from 9 RCTs with intervention periods between 5 days and 22 weeks), ranged from 0.04 to 0.94.

In 2 experimental studies, researchers examined math learning from interactive apps compared with noninteractive video, with mixed results. Younger preschoolers’ (3–4.5 years) performance on a quantity math task improved after watching an instructional video, but not after playing a math app, whereas older preschoolers (4.5–5.5 years) improved in neither condition.62  In the other study, preschoolers’ (3.5–5.5 years) performance on a measuring task improved in both interactive app and noninteractive video conditions.30 

LA Learning

In 7 randomized studies (5 RCTs) and 4 quasi-experimental studies, researchers examined children’s LA outcomes (letter knowledge, phonological awareness, letter writing, or vocabulary). Eight of the 11 studies were based in schools, whereas 3 were laboratory-based experiments. Interventions ranged from 1 week to 1 academic year, and most used commercially available apps. Overall, 3 of the 7 randomized studies (including 0 of the 3 experimental studies) and 3 of the 4 quasi-experimental studies showed better outcomes in the app intervention group compared with the control group, whereas the remainder showed statistically equivalent outcomes between groups.

Specifically, in 3 studies (2 RCTs), researchers examined alphabet knowledge and phonological awareness after children used apps targeting these skills, compared with apps not targeting these skills. No significant differences in learning were found between groups for 2 studies,44,53  whereas the other found children’s phonological awareness and vocabulary skills improved more after using a phonological awareness app compared with a vocabulary app.48  In 5 studies (2 RCTs), researchers found that children’s letter naming and writing skills50,60,77,78  and phonological awareness46,60,78  improved after using a set of LA apps compared with a classroom or in-person instruction control group. In 2 experimental studies, researchers examined children’s vocabulary learning using an app compared with nonapp instruction (noninteractive video and picture card instruction) and found no differences in learning.51,56  In 1 final study, researchers found that children’s receptive and expressive vocabulary did not improve after 2 10-minute sessions using a storytelling app with their parents compared with no treatment.52  Effect sizes for the RCTs, when reported (n = 12, from 3 school-based interventions over 1–9 weeks), ranged from 0.07 to 0.94. Effect sizes for the experimental studies, when reported (n = 3, from 2 studies), ranged from 0.41 to 0.60.

Health and Science Facts

In 3 randomized studies (1 home-based RCT and 2 experimental studies), researchers examined children’s learning of health or science concepts. In the studies, researchers examined children’s learning of healthy versus unhealthy foods, the growth cycle, and facts about animals. Overall, the 3 studies indicated children learned from the apps, with some exceptions. In 1 study, researchers found no difference in learning facts about animals between an interactive app group and a group receiving in-person instruction,58  whereas researchers in another found the app group learned more about healthy foods compared with a no-treatment control.59  In a third study, researchers found that older preschoolers (4.5–5.5 years) learned about the growth cycle better from interactive app play compared with noninteractive video instruction, but younger preschoolers (3–4.5 years) were not able to learn in either condition.62 

Executive Functioning

In 4 experimental studies, researchers examined children’s performance on executive functioning tasks. In 2 studies, researchers found that children improved their performance on the traditional three-dimensional (3D) version of the Tower of Hanoi task after practice trials with a touchscreen app version of the task, and this improvement was not significantly different from children who both practiced and were tested with the 3D version.64,66  In the third study, researchers found that young toddlers (mean age = 25 months) were better able to learn how to retrieve objects in a real-life situation when they received training with an interactive app compared with a noninteractive video; however, this effect was reversed for older toddlers (mean age = 33 months).63  In the fourth study, researchers found that children were more likely to pass a gift-delay task and performed better on a working memory task after playing an educational app game compared with watching noneducational television.65 

Social Communication

In 3 RCTs, researchers examined whether social communication skills improved after using apps designed to improve social skills, such as imitation, attention to social stimuli, and following social cues. Each study included a clinical sample of children with ASD; no studies with social development outcomes in typically developing children met inclusion criteria for this review. In these 3 studies, researchers compared outcomes in a treatment group receiving the app at home in addition to standard applied behavioral analysis or other community therapies to a control group receiving only treatment as usual. For 2 of the 3 studies, children and parents used the app together,67,72  whereas in the third study, children used the app alone.29  None of the 3 studies reported significant improvement in the primary social communication skills outcome measures for the app treatment group compared with the control group. Effect sizes for gains in the app groups on social communication outcomes ranged from 0 to 0.40.

In this article, we systematically reviewed 35 studies on young children’s learning from interactive educational apps. The studies were heterogeneous in targeted outcomes, length of intervention, context of intervention, and experimental design. Evidence for learning from touchscreen interactive app use was strongest for math-related app interventions and weakest for interventions targeting social communication skills. Study quality was mixed overall, with the risk of bias for many studies unclear in more than 1 domain, often from deficits in methods reporting.

A majority of studies comparing math or LA (letter naming and writing, phonological awareness) outcomes in children using interactive apps to controls receiving equivalent regular classroom or small-group instruction found more favorable outcomes for the interactive app condition. In addition, although study effect sizes varied widely, large effects were reported for school-based studies of math and LA learning. For example, the largest effect sizes in math were from a school-based 22-week intervention using the app Math Shelf,27  whereas the largest effect sizes in LA were from a school-based 6-week intervention using a phonological awareness app.48  These findings are notable because they suggest the addition of interactive touchscreen technology may afford a learning advantage for early academic learning exceeding traditional modes of instruction and are consistent with the theory that interactive apps are well suited to support active, repeated, and varied practice of skills.2,16 

Results of other studies reviewed suggest touchscreen app games have the potential to support learning in other skill areas, including science knowledge and executive functioning. However, there were a small number of studies targeting these skill areas, and further research is needed to establish whether children’s understanding of science concepts could be promoted using interactive apps, and similarly, whether improvement in executive functioning task performance is task specific or would generalize to other measures of executive functioning.

Results from the 3 RCTs in which researchers examined app interventions targeting social communication skills for children with ASD suggest these skills may not be best supported by touchscreen apps. This finding is particularly important to note because touchscreen technology is popular in ASD treatment, including electronic augmentative exchange communication systems.79  Social communication skills may be less easily generalized to the real world, compared with foundational early math and preliteracy skills, which is consistent with research showing improvements in app gameplay but not real-world social communication skills.29  However, given the small number of studies examining this outcome, further research in this area is needed.

Finally, findings from laboratory experimental studies on children’s learning from interactive apps compared with noninteractive video were mixed. In general, researchers in these studies examined learning in younger children who have difficulty transferring learning from media.80  Although in some studies researchers found that task performance improved with an interactive learning phase,63  others found the interactive component may have interfered with learning because of overtaxed attentional resources.62  Although other studies show older children may benefit from learning through interactive app play, further research is needed to establish the lower age limit of this potential benefit.

Important gaps were identified and should be addressed in future research. First, more large-scale randomized trials of apps are needed, with focus on clarifying what app features and content may best support learning. Thousands of apps are added to app stores every month,81  so large-scale trials of each new educational app may not be feasible. Understanding features of interactive apps supporting learning in different content areas will allow clearer standards regarding apps to be deemed “educational” by app makers.

Second, most studies reviewed were conducted in schools and examined math and LA outcomes. Given touchscreen devices are widely present in homes and the AAP’s recommendation that screen time at home be educational, it will be important to examine educational benefits of touchscreen media use at home. More studies examining other learning content areas are also needed. Third, almost all app interventions reviewed involved child solo use of apps. Children learn best when coviewing television with parents,82  and parent behaviors while co-using apps have been linked to child positive engagement and affect.83  Thus, examining children’s learning from app games with parental co-use is an important area for future research.

Fourth, all but 5 of the reviewed studies included app interventions delivered in English to English-speaking populations, and most included studies based in the United States, making it difficult to evaluate the impact of country or language on learning from apps. There was no apparent pattern to the results of studies according to country or language; for example, of the 4 studies in which researchers examined children’s math outcomes conducted outside of the United States, 3 showed better performance in the app condition, including the 1 study conducted in Greek. Further research is needed on the extent to which educational apps can support learning in a child’s nonpreferred language as well as on the generalizability to other languages of the findings presented on LA learning from apps.

Fifth, further research is needed to evaluate important moderators of treatment effects. There is emerging evidence from the studies reviewed that age may be an important moderating factor, and children below preschool age may have more difficulty learning from screen media. Future researchers should more closely examine moderating factors that may impact learning from touchscreen apps, including children’s pretest ability and demographic characteristics, such as sex, socioeconomic status, and previous experience and familiarity with technology.

Finally, in a separate body of research on television, researchers have identified negative effects of excessive screen use on children’s development.68  Although the authors of the articles in this review did not explicitly examine negative effects of use or overuse, as the popularity and diversity of digital media for children increases, it will be necessary for researchers and clinicians to balance the potential positives of interactive screen media for learning with the imperative to prevent overuse. Further research examining the relative advantages and disadvantages of various types of screen media (eg, television versus interactive touchscreen) will be important to update recommendations to better reflect the new digital context.

There were some limitations to this systematic review. First, risk of bias was unclear for many studies, and many studies had incomplete reporting of study methods. Second, although the study included unpublished research, it is possible additional unpublished research was not identified. Unpublished studies may have fewer positive outcomes as a result of publication bias.84  Third, because of the nascent state of the literature, the summarized studies were highly heterogeneous. Thus, it was not possible to compare results across studies or investigate moderators of these effects.

In this systematic review, we examined the literature on educational app learning in young children. Evidence of learning benefits from educational touchscreen apps was found across multiple studies, particularly for early mathematics. Conversely, multiple studies did not find evidence of an intervention effect for apps aiming to improve social communication skills in children with ASD, providing some preliminary evidence that concrete academic skills may be better supported by interactive apps than more-complex social-emotional skills. Findings were mixed in studies in which researchers examined children’s short-term learning from interactive touchscreen media, particularly for toddlers. With the results of this review, we highlight both the potential for interactive apps to play a useful role in promoting some types of early skills as well as the substantial gaps in knowledge that still exist regarding their use. Continued research in this area will be critical to inform the debate around young children’s screen time, as clinicians and researchers try to strike a balance between taking advantage of the potential benefits of new technology, while encouraging limits in screen time.

We thank David H. Arnold, PhD, for his helpful feedback on a draft of this article.

Dr Griffith conceptualized and designed the study, developed the data extraction instrument, adapted the risk of bias instrument, collected and coded the data, and drafted the initial manuscript; Ms Hagan participated in developing the risk of bias instrument, collected and coded the data, drafted portions of the initial manuscript, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Ms Heflin and Ms Heymann participated in developing the risk of bias instrument, collected and coded data, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Bagner conceptualized and designed the study, participated in the development of the data extraction instrument, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

FUNDING: No external funding.

COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2019-3503.

AAP

American Academy of Pediatrics

App

application

ASD

autism spectrum disorder

LA

language arts

RCT

randomized controlled trial

3D

three-dimensional

1
Rideout
V
.
The Common Sense Census: Media Use by Kids Age Zero to Eight
.
San Francisco, CA
:
Common Sense Media
;
2017
. Available at: https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/csm_zerotoeight_fullreport_release_2.pdf. Accessed March 13, 2019
2
Hirsh-Pasek
K
,
Zosh
JM
,
Golinkoff
RM
,
Gray
JH
,
Robb
MB
,
Kaufman
J
.
Putting education in “educational” apps: lessons from the science of learning
.
Psychol Sci Public Interest
.
2015
;
16
(
1
):
3
34
3
Haßler
B
,
Major
L
,
Hennessy
S
.
Tablet use in schools: a critical review of the evidence for learning outcomes
.
J Comput Assist Learn
.
2016
;
32
(
2
):
139
156
4
McCarthy
C
.
Pediatricians and television: it’s time to rethink our messaging and our efforts
.
Pediatrics
.
2013
;
131
(
3
):
589
590
5
Council on Communications and Media
.
Media and young minds
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
5
):
e20162591
6
McDonald
L
,
Wardle
J
,
Llewellyn
CH
,
van Jaarsveld
CHM
,
Fisher
A
.
Predictors of shorter sleep in early childhood
.
Sleep Med
.
2014
;
15
(
5
):
536
540
7
Cox
R
,
Skouteris
H
,
Rutherford
L
,
Fuller-Tyszkiewicz
M
,
Dell’ Aquila
D
,
Hardy
LL
.
Television viewing, television content, food intake, physical activity and body mass index: a cross-sectional study of preschool children aged 2-6 years
.
Health Promot J Austr
.
2012
;
23
(
1
):
58
62
8
Zimmerman
FJ
,
Christakis
DA
,
Meltzoff
AN
.
Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years
.
J Pediatr
.
2007
;
151
(
4
):
364
368
9
Anderson
DR
,
Bryant
J
,
Wilder
A
,
Santomero
A
,
Williams
M
,
Crawley
AM
.
Researching Blue’s Clues: viewing behavior and impact
.
Media Psychol
.
2000
;
2
(
2
):
179
194
10
Anderson
DR
,
Pempek
TA
.
Television and very young children
.
Am Behav Sci
.
2005
;
48
(
5
):
505
522
11
Anderson
DR
,
Hanson
KG
.
What researchers have learned about toddlers and television
.
Zero Three
.
2013
;
33
(
4
):
4
10
12
Whitehurst
GJ
,
Lonigan
CJ
.
Child development and emergent literacy
.
Child Dev
.
1998
;
69
(
3
):
848
872
13
Purpura
DJ
,
Baroody
AJ
,
Lonigan
CJ
.
The transition from informal to formal mathematical knowledge: mediation by numeral knowledge
.
J Educ Psychol
.
2013
;
105
(
2
):
453
464
14
Neumann
MM
,
Finger
G
,
Neumann
DL
.
A conceptual framework for emergent digital literacy
.
Early Childhood Education Journal
.
2017
;
45
(
4
):
471
479
15
Radesky
JS
,
Schumacher
J
,
Zuckerman
B
.
Mobile and interactive media use by young children: the good, the bad, and the unknown
.
Pediatrics
.
2015
;
135
(
1
):
1
3
16
Radesky
JS
,
Christakis
DA
.
Increased screen time: implications for early childhood development and behavior
.
Pediatr Clin North Am
.
2016
;
63
(
5
):
827
839
17
Xie
H
,
Peng
J
,
Qin
M
,
Huang
X
,
Tian
F
,
Zhou
Z
.
Can touchscreen devices be used to facilitate young children’s learning? A meta-analysis of touchscreen learning effect
.
Front Psychol
.
2018
;
9
:
2580
18
Fletcher
J
.
What is heterogeneity and is it important?
BMJ
.
2007
;
334
(
7584
):
94
96
19
Ryan
R
,
Cochrane Consumers and Communication Review Group
.
Cochrane consumers and communication review group: data synthesis and analysis.
Available at: https://cccrg.cochrane.org/sites/cccrg.cochrane.org/files/public/uploads/Analysis.pdf. Accessed April 13, 2019
20
Duncan
GJ
,
Dowsett
CJ
,
Claessens
A
, et al
.
School readiness and later achievement
.
Dev Psychol
.
2007
;
43
(
6
):
1428
1446
21
High
PC
,
Klass
P
;
Council on Early Childhood
.
Literacy promotion: an essential component of primary care pediatric practice
.
Pediatrics
.
2014
;
134
(
2
):
404
409
22
Randolph
J.
Free-marginal multirater kappa: an alternative to Fleiss’ fixed-marginal multirater kappa.
In:
Proceedings from the Joensuu University Learning and Instruction Symposium 2005
;
October 14–15, 2005
;
Joensuu, Finland
.
23
Warrens
MJ
.
Inequalities between multi-rater kappas
.
Adv Data Anal Classif
.
2010
;
4
(
4
):
271
286
24
Higgins
JPT
,
Altman
DG
,
Gøtzsche
PC
, et al;
Cochrane Bias Methods Group
;
Cochrane Statistical Methods Group
.
The Cochrane Collaboration’s tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials
.
BMJ
.
2011
;
343
:
d5928
25
Fleiss
JL
.
Statistical Methods for Rates and Proportions
.
Hoboken, JN
:
Wiley
;
1981
26
Schacter
J
,
Jo
B
.
Improving low-income preschoolers mathematics achievement with Math Shelf, a preschool tablet computer curriculum
.
Comput Human Behav
.
2016
;
55
:
223
229
27
Schacter
J
,
Jo
B
.
Improving preschoolers’ mathematics achievement with tablets: a randomized controlled trial
.
Math Educ Res J
.
2017
;
29
(
3
):
313
327
28
Schacter
J
,
Shih
J
,
Allen
CM
, et al
.
Math Shelf: a randomized trial of a prekindergarten tablet number sense curriculum
.
Early Educ Dev
.
2016
;
27
(
1
):
74
88
29
Fletcher-Watson
S
,
Petrou
A
,
Scott-Barrett
J
, et al
.
A trial of an iPadTM intervention targeting social communication skills in children with autism
.
Autism
.
2016
;
20
(
7
):
771
782
30
Aladé
F
,
Lauricella
AR
,
Beaudoin-Ryan
L
,
Wartella
E
.
Measuring with Murray: touchscreen technology and preschoolers’ STEM learning
.
Comput Human Behav
.
2016
;
62
:
433
441
31
Han
A
.
How Training Set and Prior Knowledge Affect Preschoolers’ Perception of Quantity and Early Number Learning [dissertation]
.
West Lafayette, IN
:
Purdue University
;
2018
32
Kosko
K
,
Ferdig
R
.
Effects of a tablet-based mathematics application for pre-school children
.
J Comput Math Sci Teach
.
2016
;
35
(
1
):
61
79
33
Mattoon
C
,
Bates
A
,
Shifflet
R
,
Latham
N
,
Ennis
S
.
Examining computational skills in prekindergarteners: the effects of traditional and digital manipulatives in a prekindergarten classroom
.
Early Child Res Pract
.
2015
;
17
(
1
)
34
Ginsburg
H
,
Baroody
A
.
Test of Early Mathematics Ability
.
Austin, TX
:
PRO-ED
;
2003
35
Miller
T
.
Developing numeracy skills using interactive technology in a play-based learning environment
.
Int J STEM Educ
.
2018
;
5
(
1
):
39
36
Outhwaite
LA
,
Faulder
M
,
Gulliford
A
,
Pitchford
NJ
.
Raising early achievement in math with interactive apps: a randomized control trial
.
J Educ Psychol
.
2019
;
111
(
2
):
284
298
37
Math Assessment Resource Service
.
Progress Test in Maths Level 5
.
London, UK
:
GL Assessment
;
2015
38
Outhwaite
LA
,
Gulliford
A
,
Pitchford
NJ
.
Closing the gap: efficacy of a tablet intervention to support the development of early mathematical skills in UK primary school children
.
Comput Educ
.
2017
;
108
:
43
58
39
Papadakis
S
,
Kalogiannakis
M
,
Zaranis
N
.
The effectiveness of computer and tablet assisted intervention in early childhood students’ understanding of numbers. An empirical study conducted in Greece
.
Educ Inf Technol
.
2018
;
23
(
5
):
1849
1871
40
Park
J
,
Bermudez
V
,
Roberts
RC
,
Brannon
EM
.
Non-symbolic approximate arithmetic training improves math performance in preschoolers
.
J Exp Child Psychol
.
2016
;
152
:
278
293
41
Spencer
P.
iPads: improving numeracy learning in the early years.
In:
Steinle
V
,
Ball
L
,
Bardini
C
, eds.
Proceedings from the 36th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia
;
July 7–11, 2013
;
Melbourne, Australia
42
Blackwell
CK
. Technology use in early childhood education: investigating teacher access and attitudes toward technology and the effect of iPads on student achievement [dissertation]. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University;
2016
43
Renaissance
Learning
.
STAR Early Literacy Accessment.
Available at: https://www.renaissance.com/products/assessment/star-360/star-early-literacy-skills/. Accessed November 1, 2019
44
Brown
M
,
Harmon
MT
.
iPad intervention with at-risk preschoolers: mobile technology in the classroom
.
J Lit Technol
.
2013
;
14
(
2
):
56
78
45
Invernezzi
M
,
Sullivan
A
,
Meier
J
.
Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening - Prekindergarten
.
Charlottesville, VA
:
University Press
;
2001
46
Cubelic
CC
,
Larwin
KH
.
The use of iPad technology in the kindergarten classroom: a quasi-experimental investigation of the impact on early literacy skills
.
Compr J Educ Res
.
2014
;
2
(
4
):
47
59
47
Good
RH
,
Kaminski
RA
.
Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills Administration and Scoring Guide
, 6th ed.
Eugene, OR
:
Institute for the Development of Educational Achievement
;
2003
48
Gilliver
M
,
Cupples
L
,
Ching
TYC
,
Leigh
G
,
Gunnourie
M
.
Developing sound skills for reading: teaching phonological awareness to preschoolers with hearing loss
.
J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ
.
2016
;
21
(
3
):
268
279
49
Neumann
MM
.
Using tablets and apps to enhance emergent literacy skills in young children
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2018
;
42
:
239
246
50
Patchan
MM
,
Puranik
CS
.
Using tablet computers to teach preschool children to write letters: exploring the impact of extrinsic and intrinsic feedback
.
Comput Educ
.
2016
;
102
:
128
137
51
Russo-Johnson
C
,
Troseth
G
,
Duncan
C
,
Mesghina
A
.
All tapped out: touchscreen interactivity and young children’s word learning
.
Front Psychol
.
2017
;
8
:
578
52
Teepe
RC
,
Molenaar
I
,
Verhoeven
L
.
Technology-enhanced storytelling stimulating parent–child interaction and preschool children’s vocabulary knowledge
.
J Comput Assist Learn
.
2017
;
33
(
2
):
123
136
53
Vatalaro
A
,
Mcdonald
A
,
Debbie
C
,
Vaughn
LH
,
Barnes
AC
.
A quasi-experiment examining expressive and receptive vocabulary knowledge of preschool head start children using mobile media apps
.
Early Child Educ J
.
2018
;
46
(
4
):
451
466
54
Dunn
L
,
Dunn
D.
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test
. 4th Ed.
Minneapolis, MN
:
NCS Pearson
;
2007
55
Williams
K
.
EVT-2
.
Minneapolis, MN
:
Pearson Assessments
;
2007
56
Walter‐Laager
C
,
Brandenberg
K
,
Tinguely
L
,
Schwarz
J
,
Pfiffner
MR
,
Moschner
B
.
Media‐assisted language learning for young children: effects of a word‐learning app on the vocabulary acquisition of two‐year‐olds
.
Br J Educ Technol
.
2017
;
48
(
4
):
1062
1072
57
Grimm
H
,
Aktaş
M
,
Frevert
S
.
SETK-2
.
Göttingen, Germany
:
Hogrefe
;
2000
58
Kwok
K
,
Ghrear
S
,
Li
V
,
Haddock
T
,
Coleman
P
,
Birch
SAJ
.
Children can learn new facts equally well from interactive media versus face to face instruction
.
Front Psychol
.
2016
;
7
:
1603
59
Putnam
MM
,
Richmond
EM
,
Brunick
KL
,
Wright
CA
,
Calvert
SL
.
Influence of a character-based app on children’s learning of nutritional information: should apps be served with a side of media characters?
Games Health J
.
2018
;
7
(
2
):
121
126
60
Bebell
D
,
Pedulla
J
.
A quantitative investigation into the impacts of 1:1 iPads on early learners’ ELA and math achievement
.
J Inf Technol Educ Innov Pract
.
2015
;
14
:
191
215
61
NWEA
.
Children’s Progress Academic Assessment.
2009
. Available at: https://www.nwea.org/cpaa/. Accessed November 12, 2019
62
Schroeder
EL
,
Kirkorian
HL
.
When seeing is better than doing: preschoolers’ transfer of STEM skills using touchscreen games
.
Front Psychol
.
2016
;
7
:
1377
63
Choi
K
,
Kirkorian
HL
.
Touch or watch to learn? Toddlers’ object retrieval using contingent and noncontingent video
.
Psychol Sci
.
2016
;
27
(
5
):
726
736
64
Huber
B
,
Tarasuik
J
,
Antoniou
MN
,
Garrett
C
,
Bowe
SJ
,
Kaufman
J
.
Young children’s transfer of learning from a touchscreen device
.
Comput Human Behav
.
2016
;
56
:
56
64
65
Huber
B
,
Yeates
M
,
Meyer
D
,
Fleckhammer
L
,
Kaufman
J
.
The effects of screen media content on young children’s executive functioning
.
J Exp Child Psychol
.
2018
;
170
:
72
85
66
Tarasuik
J
,
Demaria
A
,
Kaufman
J
.
Transfer of problem solving skills from touchscreen to 3D model by 3- to 6-year-olds
.
Front Psychol
.
2017
;
8
:
1586
67
Esposito
M
,
Sloan
J
,
Tancredi
A
, et al
.
Using tablet applications for children with autism to increase their cognitive and social skills
.
J Spec Educ Technol
.
2017
;
32
(
4
):
199
209
68
Fenson
L
,
Dale
P
,
Reznick
J
, et al
.
Macarthur Communicative Development Inventory: User’s Guide and Technical Manual
.
San Diego, CA
:
Singular Publishing Company
;
1993
69
Wetherby
A
,
Prizant
B
.
Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales: Developmental Profile
.
Baltimore, MD
:
Paul H. Brooks Pub.
;
2002
70
Grzadzinski
R
,
Carr
T
,
Colombi
C
, et al
.
Measuring changes in social communication behaviors: preliminary development of the Brief Observation of Social Communication Change (BOSCC)
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2016
;
46
(
7
):
2464
2479
71
Lord
C
,
Rutter
M
,
DiLavore
P
,
Risi
S
.
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2)
.
Los Angeles, CA
:
WPS
;
2012
72
Whitehouse
AJO
,
Granich
J
,
Alvares
G
, et al
.
A randomised controlled trial of an iPad-based application to complement early behavioural intervention in Autism Spectrum Disorder
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2017
;
58
(
9
):
1042
1052
73
Bodfish
J
,
Symons
F
,
Lewis
M
.
The Repetitive Behaviour Scale: Test Manual
.
Morganton, NC
:
Western California Center Research Reports
;
1998
74
Peters-Scheffer
N
,
Didden
R
,
Green
V
, et al
.
The behavior flexibility rating scale-revised (BFRS-R): Factor analysis, internal consistency, inter-rater and intra-rater reliability, and convergent validity
.
Res Dev Disabil
.
2008
;
29
(
5
):
398
407
75
Rimland
B
,
Edelson
S
.
Autism Treatment Evaluation Checklist: Statistical Analyses
.
San Diego, CA
:
Autism Research Institute
;
2000
76
Sparrow
S
,
Cicchetti
D
,
Balla
D
.
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales
, 2nd Edition Manual.
Minneapolis, MN
:
NCS Pearson
;
2005
77
Neumann
MM
.
Using tablets and apps to enhance emergent literacy skills in young children
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2018
;
42
:
239
246
78
Blackwell
CK
.
Technology Use in Early Childhood Education: Investigating Teacher Access and Attitudes Toward Technology and the Effect of IPads on Student Achievement [dissertation]
.
Vol 76
.
Evanston, IL
:
Northwestern University
;
2016
79
Alzrayer
N
,
Banda
DR
,
Koul
RK
.
Use of iPad/iPods with individuals with autism and other developmental disabilities: a meta-analysis of communication interventions
.
Rev J Autism Dev Disord
.
2014
;
1
(
3
):
179
191
80
Zack
E
,
Gerhardstein
P
,
Meltzoff
AN
,
Barr
R
.
15-month-olds’ transfer of learning between touch screen and real-world displays: language cues and cognitive loads
.
Scand J Psychol
.
2013
;
54
(
1
):
20
25
81
42matters
.
Google Play Store stats
. Available at: https://42matters.com/stats. Accessed July 12, 2019
82
Strouse
GA
,
O’Doherty
K
,
Troseth
GL
.
Effective coviewing: preschoolers’ learning from video after a dialogic questioning intervention
.
Dev Psychol
.
2013
;
49
(
12
):
2368
2382
83
Griffith
SF
,
Arnold
DH
.
Home learning in the new mobile age: parent–child interactions during joint play with educational apps in the US
.
J Child Media
.
2019
;
13
(
1
):
1
19
84
Rosenthal
R
.
The “file drawer problem” and tolerance for null results
.
Psychol Bull
.
1979
;
86
(
3
):
638
641

Competing Interests

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.