The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends infant-parent room-sharing until age 1. We assessed the association between room-sharing and sleep outcomes.
The Intervention Nurses Start Infants Growing on Healthy Trajectories study is an obesity prevention trial comparing a responsive parenting intervention with a safety control among primiparous mother-infant dyads. Mothers completed the Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire at 4, 9, 12, and 30 months. Reported sleep duration and overnight behaviors, adjusted for intervention group, were compared among early independent sleepers (own room <4 months), later independent sleepers (own room between 4 and 9 months), and room-sharers at 9 months.
At 4 months, reported overnight sleep duration was similar between groups, but compared with room-sharers, early independent sleepers had better sleep consolidation (longest stretch: 46 more minutes, P = .02). At 9 months, early independent sleepers slept 40 more minutes nightly than room-sharers and 26 more minutes than later independent sleepers (P = .008). The longest stretch for early independent sleepers was 100 and 45 minutes more than room-sharers and later independent sleepers, respectively (P = .01). At 30 months, infants sleeping independently by 9 months slept >45 more minutes nightly than those room-sharing at 9 months (P = .004). Room-sharers had 4 times the odds of transitioning to bed-sharing overnight at both 4 and 9 months (P < .01 for both).
Room-sharing at ages 4 and 9 months is associated with less nighttime sleep in both the short and long-term, reduced sleep consolidation, and unsafe sleep practices previously associated with sleep-related death.
The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends infant-parent room-sharing through age 1. Short sleep duration during infancy has been associated with adverse outcomes for infants and families. The effect of room-sharing on sleep outcomes has been inadequately explored.
Room-sharing at ages 4 and 9 months is associated with less nighttime sleep, shorter sleep stretches, and unsafe sleep practices previously associated with sleep-related death.
The importance of getting an adequate night’s sleep has been increasingly recognized by professional societies including the American Academy of Pediatrics1 (AAP) and the American Academy of Sleep Medicine.2 Inadequate sleep has been associated with poorer cognitive, psychomotor, physical, and socioemotional development, which includes emotion regulation, mood, and behavior in infancy and childhood.3,–6 Insufficient sleep is generally accepted as an important risk factor for obesity,7,8 with links between sleep and weight beginning in infancy.9,–14 Short sleep duration during infancy is also associated with inadequate sleep later in childhood,15 emphasizing the importance of developing healthy sleep-related behaviors during the first months after birth. Also, infant sleep has a bidirectional relationship with parent outcomes as demonstrated by associations between infant sleep and parental sleep, maternal sensitivity, relationship quality, parental emotional health, and parenting practices.16,–18
The desire to optimize infant sleep duration and consolidation, however, must be balanced with safe infant sleep, a fact reinforced by the 3500 infants who tragically die of sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) or other sleep-related deaths annually.19 According to the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development’s “Safe to Sleep” campaign, most SIDS deaths occur when infants are 1 to 4 months old, 90% occurring before the age of 6 months.20 Despite these figures, the recently published AAP Policy Statement, SIDS and Other Sleep-Related Infant Deaths, recommended that infants sleep in their parents’ room on a separate surface, ideally for the entire first year but at least for the first 6 months.21 The 1-year recommendation has questionable congruence with the epidemiology of SIDS (as risk is far lower after 6 months), and it runs counter to the common clinical advice parents receive. Based on evidence of improved infant sleep,22,–24 clinicians may encourage parents to establish independent sleep environments (ie, in a separate room from parents) during the middle of the first year to promote healthy and sustainable sleep patterns before the typical onset of separation anxiety later in the first year.25 Therefore, in this secondary data analysis, we sought to evaluate the associations between room-sharing on a separate sleep surface and sleep-related outcomes from the cohort participating in the Intervention Nurses Start Infants Growing on Healthy Trajectories (INSIGHT) study. We hypothesized that room-sharing would be associated with shorter nighttime sleep duration and problematic sleep-related behaviors during infancy when compared with independent sleeping in a separate room. We also sought to examine risk factors for SIDS among infants who were and were not independent sleepers.
Methods
Participants and Design
As previously described,26,–28 mothers and their newborns were recruited after delivery from one maternity ward (Penn State Milton S. Hershey Medical Center, Hershey, PA) for participation in the INSIGHT study from January 2012 to March 2014. The study design and inclusion and exclusion criteria have been previously published.26 Major eligibility criteria specified that the subjects be term singletons weighing ≥2500 g delivered to English-speaking, primiparous mothers ≥20 years old. Dyads were randomly assigned 2 weeks after delivery to a responsive parenting (RP) intervention or a safety control group. 279 mother-infant dyads completed the first home visit and are considered the study cohort for outcomes as specified before study initiation in the protocol. For the current analyses, those parents who reported bed-sharing with their newborn were excluded because the goal was to compare those who were sleeping independently in a separate room from parents with those still room-sharing but not bed-sharing (as recommended by the AAP).
Research nurses conducted home visits at infant ages of 3 to 4 weeks and 4, 6, and 9 months. Research center visits occurred at 1 and 2 years of age. Intervention components have been previously described.27 Among other sleep-related topics discussed at the 3- to 4-week home visit, the RP group received guidance encouraging parents to consider moving their child’s sleep location at the age of 3 months to the room they would prefer the infant to sleep in at 1 year of age, with the advice that such a move would be more difficult if the family waited much beyond that point.25 This point was reinforced at the 4-month home visit. The safety control group received education regarding prevention of SIDS, which included direct observation of the sleep environment by the visiting nurses with individual feedback to improve safety at the first 3 home visits. This study was approved by the Penn State College of Medicine’s Human Subjects Protection Office and registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov.
Measures
Background Characteristics
Demographic information collected at enrollment included parent and child race/ethnicity, maternal education, annual income, and marital status. Data extracted from medical charts included maternal age, prepregnancy weight, gestational weight gain, infant gestational age, and birth anthropometrics.
Sleep
The validated Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire was used to assess sleep at 4 and 9 months of age with an abbreviated version administered at 12 and 30 months.30 This survey assesses infant sleep location, before-bedtime activities, and sleep patterns. Sleep duration is divided into nighttime (7 pm–7 am) and daytime (7 am–7 pm) and is reported in hours and minutes. Additional questions regarding sleep location, night waking, night feedings, bedtime routine components and duration, infant sleep behaviors and environment, and parenting responses to night wakings were asked at 4 and 9 months. Instructions for the infant sleep survey guided mothers to think about her child’s sleep during the past week when answering the questions and base responses on a “typical night.” Maternal sleep quality was measured at 4 months of infant age by using the PROMIS Sleep Disturbance instrument (PROMIS Network, American Institutes for Research, Chapel Hill, NC).31
Other
Feeding mode was assessed by using a food frequency questionnaire. Infants were categorized as predominantly breastfed if ≥80% of milk feedings were breastmilk.32
Statistical Analysis
Data were analyzed by using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). Descriptive statistics were generated for sleep duration and behaviors on the basis of the timing of our 2 sleep assessments during infancy at 4 and 9 months of age, which created 3 groups: early independent sleepers (own room <4 months), later independent sleepers (own room between 4 and 9 months), and room-sharers at 9 months. For continuous outcomes, differences by sleep location were assessed by using analysis of variance (ANOVA) at each of the 4 times, adjusting for study group assignment. Categorical outcomes were assessed by using logistic regression, adjusting for study group. Analyses including socioeconomic covariates (eg, income, living in a single-family home) did not result in major differences in results, and thus results from models that included only study group are presented. Statistical significance was defined as P ≤ .05.
Results
Among 279 participants,27,28 259 (93%) remained in the study through 9 months of age. Among this group, 20 were bed-sharing at 4 and/or 9 months and 9 had missing data on sleep. These participants were excluded from further analyses, leaving a sample of 230 mother-infant pairs as the sample for this manuscript. The majority of infants, 142 (62%), were “early independent sleepers,” sleeping independently without room-sharing by 4 months (Table 1). Another 62 (27%) were “later independent sleepers” who began sleeping independently between 4 and 9 months of age, whereas 26 (11%) were room-sharing at 9 months. There were no differences between intervention groups in terms of frequency of bed-sharing or room-sharing at 4 or 9 months. Room-sharing was associated with demographic variables including race/ethnicity, lower income and education, unmarried and/or not cohabiting with partner, fewer bedrooms in the home, and extended family and/or other people living in the home.
Differences in Maternal Demographics and Home Characteristics by Mother-Infant Sleeping Arrangements
. | Early Independent Sleepers . | Later Independent Sleepers . | Still Room-Sharing at 9 mo . | P . |
---|---|---|---|---|
N = 142 . | N = 62 . | N = 26a . | ||
Maternal demographics | ||||
Maternal age, y (SD) | 29.1 (3.9) | 29.8 (4.9) | 28.1 (6.4) | .25 |
Prepregnancy BMI (SD) | 26.0 (5.5) | 24.7 (4.3) | 26.0 (7.1) | .28 |
Race/ethnicity, N (%) | .001 | |||
White, non-Hispanic | 135 (95) | 54 (87) | 19 (73) | |
Other | 7 (5) | 8 (13) | 7 (27) | |
Annual household income, N (%) | .01 | |||
<$25 000 | 8 (6) | 5 (8) | 5 (19) | |
$25 000–$49 999 | 13 (9) | 3 (5) | 4 (15) | |
$50 000–$74 999 | 41 (29) | 14 (23) | 9 (35) | |
≥$75 000 | 77 (54) | 36 (58) | 5 (19) | |
Don’t know or declined to answer | 3 (2) | 4 (6) | 3 (12) | |
Education, N (%) | <.001 | |||
High school graduate or less | 10 (7) | 4 (6) | 4 (15) | |
Some college | 26 (18) | 18 (29) | 14 (54) | |
College graduate or higher | 106 (75) | 40 (65) | 8 (31) | |
Married and/or living with partner, N (%) | 138 (97) | 59 (95) | 21 (81) | .004 |
Returned to work ≤12 wk postpartum, N (%) | 72 (51) | 29 (48) | 19 (73) | .07 |
Predominantly breastfeeding, N (%) | ||||
At 1 mo | 82 (58) | 47 (77) | 14 (56) | .03 |
At 4 mo | 64 (45) | 36 (58) | 11 (42) | .21 |
At 6 mo | 52 (37) | 29 (47) | 6 (24) | .12 |
At 9 mo | 46 (32) | 24 (39) | 7 (27) | .51 |
Home characteristics | ||||
Single-family home, N (%) | 102 (72) | 38 (63) | 14 (56) | .20 |
Number of bedrooms, N (%) | <.001 | |||
1 | 0 (0) | 2 (3) | 3 (12) | |
2 | 27 (19) | 18 (29) | 8 (31) | |
3+ | 115 (81) | 42 (68) | 15 (58) | |
Extended family and/or others living in home, N (%) | 16 (12) | 8 (14) | 10 (42) | .005 |
. | Early Independent Sleepers . | Later Independent Sleepers . | Still Room-Sharing at 9 mo . | P . |
---|---|---|---|---|
N = 142 . | N = 62 . | N = 26a . | ||
Maternal demographics | ||||
Maternal age, y (SD) | 29.1 (3.9) | 29.8 (4.9) | 28.1 (6.4) | .25 |
Prepregnancy BMI (SD) | 26.0 (5.5) | 24.7 (4.3) | 26.0 (7.1) | .28 |
Race/ethnicity, N (%) | .001 | |||
White, non-Hispanic | 135 (95) | 54 (87) | 19 (73) | |
Other | 7 (5) | 8 (13) | 7 (27) | |
Annual household income, N (%) | .01 | |||
<$25 000 | 8 (6) | 5 (8) | 5 (19) | |
$25 000–$49 999 | 13 (9) | 3 (5) | 4 (15) | |
$50 000–$74 999 | 41 (29) | 14 (23) | 9 (35) | |
≥$75 000 | 77 (54) | 36 (58) | 5 (19) | |
Don’t know or declined to answer | 3 (2) | 4 (6) | 3 (12) | |
Education, N (%) | <.001 | |||
High school graduate or less | 10 (7) | 4 (6) | 4 (15) | |
Some college | 26 (18) | 18 (29) | 14 (54) | |
College graduate or higher | 106 (75) | 40 (65) | 8 (31) | |
Married and/or living with partner, N (%) | 138 (97) | 59 (95) | 21 (81) | .004 |
Returned to work ≤12 wk postpartum, N (%) | 72 (51) | 29 (48) | 19 (73) | .07 |
Predominantly breastfeeding, N (%) | ||||
At 1 mo | 82 (58) | 47 (77) | 14 (56) | .03 |
At 4 mo | 64 (45) | 36 (58) | 11 (42) | .21 |
At 6 mo | 52 (37) | 29 (47) | 6 (24) | .12 |
At 9 mo | 46 (32) | 24 (39) | 7 (27) | .51 |
Home characteristics | ||||
Single-family home, N (%) | 102 (72) | 38 (63) | 14 (56) | .20 |
Number of bedrooms, N (%) | <.001 | |||
1 | 0 (0) | 2 (3) | 3 (12) | |
2 | 27 (19) | 18 (29) | 8 (31) | |
3+ | 115 (81) | 42 (68) | 15 (58) | |
Extended family and/or others living in home, N (%) | 16 (12) | 8 (14) | 10 (42) | .005 |
Includes 5 infants who were not room-sharing at 4 mo.
Reported Sleep Duration, Consolidation, Night Wakings, and Night Feedings
At 4 months, no significant differences in nighttime or total sleep duration were observed between those who were early independent sleepers and those who room-shared at 4 and/or 9 months (Fig 1). However, the early independent sleepers had better sleep consolidation at 4 months of age with a longest (mean ± SD) sleep stretch averaging 469 ± 139 minutes compared with 423 ± 158 minutes for those who were room-sharing (Table 2) (P = .02). While the mean number of night wakings was similar between early independent and nonindependent sleeping infants at 4 months of age, those sleeping independently had fewer night feedings (1.1 vs 1.4; P = .02). Nonindependent sleepers also had twice the odds of being fed back to sleep overnight compared with independent sleepers (adjusted odds ratio [aOR] 1.95, 95% confidence interval [CI] 1.09–3.50).
Room-sharing group and nighttime sleep duration at 4, 9, 12, and 30 months of age. * P < .01.
Room-sharing group and nighttime sleep duration at 4, 9, 12, and 30 months of age. * P < .01.
Sleep Outcome Differences Between Infants Who Are Independently Sleeping by 4 mo Versus Those Who Are Not
. | Early Independent Sleepers . | Later Independent Sleepers . | Still Room-Sharing at 9 mo . | Early Independent Sleepers Versus Other Groups . | Pa . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
N = 142 . | N = 62 . | N = 26b . | aOR (95% CI) . | ||
4-mo assessment | |||||
Bedtime routine, N (%) | |||||
Consistent bedtime routine | 53 (37) | 12 (20) | 8 (32) | 1.93 (1.05–3.53) | .03 |
Falls asleep while feeding | 72 (51) | 41 (66) | 10 (40) | 0.76 (0.44–1.31) | .32 |
Put to bed drowsy but awake | 61 (43) | 19 (27) | 7 (28) | 1.70 (0.96–3.03) | .07 |
Bedtime 8 pm or earlier | 60 (42) | 18 (22) | 5 (20) | 1.93 (1.06–3.53) | .03 |
Longest sleep stretch, min (SD) | 469 (139) | 415 (154) | 440 (165) | — | .02 |
Night wakings, mean ± SD | 1.1 (1.1) | 1.4 (1.0) | 1.3 (0.8) | — | .14 |
Night feedings, mean ± SD | 1.1 (1.1) | 1.4 (1.0) | 1.4 (1.2) | — | .02 |
Fed back to sleep, N (%) | 78 (55) | 46 (74) | 16 (64) | 0.51 (0.29–0.92) | .02 |
Sleep safety, N (%) | |||||
Nonsupine sleep position | 12 (9) | 3 (5) | 4 (16) | 1.03 (0.39–2.72) | .96 |
Unapproved object on sleep surface | 48 (34) | 29 (48) | 14 (56) | 0.49 (0.28–0.85) | .02 |
Brought to parent bed overnight | 7 (5) | 9 (15) | 6 (24) | 0.24 (0.09–0.61) | .003 |
Maternal sleep poor or very poor, N (%) | 11 (8) | 10 (17) | 3 (12) | 0.47 (0.20–1.11) | .09 |
. | Early Independent Sleepers . | Later Independent Sleepers . | Still Room-Sharing at 9 mo . | Early Independent Sleepers Versus Other Groups . | Pa . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
N = 142 . | N = 62 . | N = 26b . | aOR (95% CI) . | ||
4-mo assessment | |||||
Bedtime routine, N (%) | |||||
Consistent bedtime routine | 53 (37) | 12 (20) | 8 (32) | 1.93 (1.05–3.53) | .03 |
Falls asleep while feeding | 72 (51) | 41 (66) | 10 (40) | 0.76 (0.44–1.31) | .32 |
Put to bed drowsy but awake | 61 (43) | 19 (27) | 7 (28) | 1.70 (0.96–3.03) | .07 |
Bedtime 8 pm or earlier | 60 (42) | 18 (22) | 5 (20) | 1.93 (1.06–3.53) | .03 |
Longest sleep stretch, min (SD) | 469 (139) | 415 (154) | 440 (165) | — | .02 |
Night wakings, mean ± SD | 1.1 (1.1) | 1.4 (1.0) | 1.3 (0.8) | — | .14 |
Night feedings, mean ± SD | 1.1 (1.1) | 1.4 (1.0) | 1.4 (1.2) | — | .02 |
Fed back to sleep, N (%) | 78 (55) | 46 (74) | 16 (64) | 0.51 (0.29–0.92) | .02 |
Sleep safety, N (%) | |||||
Nonsupine sleep position | 12 (9) | 3 (5) | 4 (16) | 1.03 (0.39–2.72) | .96 |
Unapproved object on sleep surface | 48 (34) | 29 (48) | 14 (56) | 0.49 (0.28–0.85) | .02 |
Brought to parent bed overnight | 7 (5) | 9 (15) | 6 (24) | 0.24 (0.09–0.61) | .003 |
Maternal sleep poor or very poor, N (%) | 11 (8) | 10 (17) | 3 (12) | 0.47 (0.20–1.11) | .09 |
Reported P values are comparing the first column (independent sleeping by 4 mo) versus the other 2 combined, adjusted for study group by using ANOVA for continuous outcomes and logistic regression for categorical outcomes.
Includes 5 infants who were not room-sharing at 4 mo.
By 9 months of age, significant differences in nighttime sleep duration emerged between groups (Table 3). The early independent sleepers slept on average 627 ± 67 minutes per night compared with 601 ± 73 minutes for later independent sleepers and 587 ± 83 minutes for those who were room-sharing at 9 months of age (P = .008). Those who were early independent sleepers had the best sleep consolidation with the longest sleep stretches, averaging 542 ± 162 minutes versus 497 ± 166 minutes for later independent sleepers and 442 ± 158 minutes for room-sharers (P = .01). Although night wakings, night feedings, and odds of being fed back to sleep overnight did not differ among groups at 9 months of age, total daily sleep duration was longest among early independent sleepers (796 ± 86 vs 760 ± 94 vs 773 ± 96 minutes, P = .04).
Sleep Outcomes at 9 mo by Timing of Independent Sleeping
. | Early Independent Sleepers . | Later Independent Sleepers . | Still Room-Sharing at 9 mo . | P (Main Effect of Room-Sharing Category)a . | Independent Sleepers Versus Room Sharers, aOR (95% CI) . | Pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N = 142 . | N = 62 . | N = 26c . | ||||
9 mo assessment | ||||||
Bedtime routine, N (%) | ||||||
Consistent bedtime routine | 67 (47) | 19 (31) | 8 (31) | .06 | 1.65 (0.68–3.99) | .27 |
Falls asleep while feeding | 51 (36) | 26 (42) | 13 (50) | .41 | 0.59 (0.25–1.36) | .22 |
Put to bed drowsy, but awake | 115 (81) | 42 (68) | 16 (62) | .05 | 2.16 (0.90–5.19) | .08 |
Bedtime 8 pm or earlier | 93 (65) | 34 (55) | 10 (38) | .03 | 2.79 (1.18–6.61) | .02 |
Longest sleep stretch, min (SD) | 542 (162) | 497 (166) | 442 (158) | .01 | — | — |
Night wakings, mean ± SD | 1.0 (1.0) | 1.0 (0.9) | 1.4 (1.2) | .10 | — | — |
Night feedings, mean ± SD | 0.5 (0.9) | 0.7 (0.9) | 0.8 (0.9) | .19 | — | — |
Fed back to sleep, N (%) | 36 (25) | 24 (39) | 11 (42) | .10 | 0.56 (0.24–1.30) | .17 |
Sleep safety, N (%) | ||||||
Nonsupine sleep position | 116 (82) | 51 (82) | 21 (84) | .96 | 0.86 (0.28–2.67) | .80 |
Unapproved object on sleep surface | 97 (69) | 41 (67) | 14 (54) | .33 | 1.89 (0.82–4.37) | .14 |
Brought to parent bed overnight | 13 (9) | 11 (18) | 9 (35) | .004 | 0.25 (0.01–0.63) | .003 |
. | Early Independent Sleepers . | Later Independent Sleepers . | Still Room-Sharing at 9 mo . | P (Main Effect of Room-Sharing Category)a . | Independent Sleepers Versus Room Sharers, aOR (95% CI) . | Pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
N = 142 . | N = 62 . | N = 26c . | ||||
9 mo assessment | ||||||
Bedtime routine, N (%) | ||||||
Consistent bedtime routine | 67 (47) | 19 (31) | 8 (31) | .06 | 1.65 (0.68–3.99) | .27 |
Falls asleep while feeding | 51 (36) | 26 (42) | 13 (50) | .41 | 0.59 (0.25–1.36) | .22 |
Put to bed drowsy, but awake | 115 (81) | 42 (68) | 16 (62) | .05 | 2.16 (0.90–5.19) | .08 |
Bedtime 8 pm or earlier | 93 (65) | 34 (55) | 10 (38) | .03 | 2.79 (1.18–6.61) | .02 |
Longest sleep stretch, min (SD) | 542 (162) | 497 (166) | 442 (158) | .01 | — | — |
Night wakings, mean ± SD | 1.0 (1.0) | 1.0 (0.9) | 1.4 (1.2) | .10 | — | — |
Night feedings, mean ± SD | 0.5 (0.9) | 0.7 (0.9) | 0.8 (0.9) | .19 | — | — |
Fed back to sleep, N (%) | 36 (25) | 24 (39) | 11 (42) | .10 | 0.56 (0.24–1.30) | .17 |
Sleep safety, N (%) | ||||||
Nonsupine sleep position | 116 (82) | 51 (82) | 21 (84) | .96 | 0.86 (0.28–2.67) | .80 |
Unapproved object on sleep surface | 97 (69) | 41 (67) | 14 (54) | .33 | 1.89 (0.82–4.37) | .14 |
Brought to parent bed overnight | 13 (9) | 11 (18) | 9 (35) | .004 | 0.25 (0.01–0.63) | .003 |
P value is for main effect of room-sharing category, controlling for study group by using ANOVA for continuous outcomes and logistic regression for categorical outcomes.
P value is for odds ratio for early independent sleepers and later independent sleepers combined compared with room-sharers as a reference group, controlling for the study group.
Includes 5 infants who were not room-sharing at 4 mo.
At 12 months of age, no differences existed between groups for nighttime or total daily sleep duration. However, the 30-month assessment revealed that both early and later independent sleepers slept on average more than 45 minutes longer at night than those who were room-sharing at 9 months (614 ± 51 vs 617 ± 70 vs 569 ± 79; P = .004). Total daily sleep duration was similar among groups at 30 months of age.
Bedtime Routines
Significant differences existed for bedtime routines at 4 months of age. Early independent sleepers had increased odds of having a consistent bedtime routine (aOR 1.93, 95% CI 1.05–3.53) and being put to bed by 8 pm (aOR 1.93, 95% CI 1.06–3.53) relative to room-sharers. At 9 months of age, room-sharing was associated with greater odds of having a bedtime after 8 pm; room-sharers also had lower odds than independent sleepers of being put to sleep drowsy but awake (P ≤ .05 for both).
Sleep Safety
At 4 months of age, room-sharing infants had greater odds of having an unapproved object (blankets, pillows, positioners, etc) on their sleep surface than those who were sleeping independently (aOR 2.04, 95% CI 1.17–3.57). Additionally, room-sharing parents with infants at 4 months of age had over 4 times the odds of bringing their infant into their bed overnight than those parenting early independent sleepers (aOR 4.24, 95% CI 1.64–10.95). Similarly, at 9 months of age, room-sharing parents had 4 times the odds than those who were sleeping independently of bringing their infant into their bed after waking overnight (aOR 3.97, 95% CI 1.59–9.90).
Discussion
These secondary analyses from the INSIGHT study demonstrate that infant-parent room-sharing at 4 and 9 months of age is associated with less reported nighttime sleep in both the short- and long-term, worse sleep consolidation, and unsafe sleep practices previously associated with sleep-related death. The findings raise questions about the well-intended AAP recommendation that room-sharing should ideally occur for all infants until their first birthday.21
The recommendation to room-share until the age of 1 year is not evidence-based for the prevention of SIDS, given that 90% of these tragedies occur before the age of 6 months.20 The data presented by the AAP Task Force on Sudden Infant Death Syndrome to support the recommendation21 came from 3 European case-control reports.33,–35 In the first by Blair et al,33 infants who died of SIDS at over 4 months of age were more likely than controls to be room-sharing without bed-sharing (46% vs 41%) and less likely to be sleeping independently in their own rooms (43% vs 53%; statistical comparison not performed), yet the age stratification presented in this paper was not acknowledged in a recently published letter by the Task Force.36 The second report involved mostly infants <6 months old and used data from the early- to mid-1990s, when prone sleeping was still common.34 The third study involved only 123 cases of SIDS with a mean age of 13.6 ± 9.5 weeks, indicating that most cases included infants <6 months old.35 This small study included only 15 infants who slept in their own room and died of SIDS. Therefore, it is unlikely that any conclusions can be made about those who were 6 to 12 months old on the basis of this small sample size and the epidemiology of the condition. Importantly, the biggest risk factor for SIDS in this third report was “sharing the same sleep surface as the last sleep location,” something that more commonly occurred among infants who initially shared their mother’s room in our study.
In addition to the lack of data supporting the recommendation to room-share until 1 year of age, the Policy Statement authored by the Task Force did not describe a mechanism by which room-sharing would prevent SIDS or how parents should apply this recommendation to achieve better outcomes than those parents choosing to have their infants sleep independently. Although the Task Force’s recent letter cited more frequent arousals among room-sharers as the potential protective mechanism,36 the 2 studies referenced do not support that conclusion. The first is a case-control study of 18 infants.37 Among 9 room-sharers, 5 were bed-sharers, making conclusions about arousals among those who are room-sharing but not bed-sharing (versus independent sleepers) problematic. The second study, which also included bed-sharers in the room-sharing group, states that parental presence in the room at bedtime (eg, holding, rocking, feeding to sleep) explained much of the variance in night wakings and that night-long room-sharing had a “negligible (<1%) independent contribution to the explained variance.”22
The AAP recommendation to room-share until the age of 1 year conflicts with sleep expert guidance,25 which recognizes developmental changes that occur over the first year. The suggestion that parents wait to move the infant out of their bedroom until the end of the first year, when separation anxiety is normative and increasing, is likely to result in frustrated parents and unhappy infants. It also conflicts with other data that room-sharing is associated with more sleep disruption for mothers.24,38
Our data showing shorter night sleep and shorter sleep bouts for infants who are room-sharing has potentially important consequences. Inadequate sleep affects nearly every health domain, including emotional regulation, mood, and behavior in infancy and childhood.3,–6 As demonstrated in our analyses, room-sharing and short sleep duration during infancy are also associated with inadequate sleep later in childhood,15 which emphasizes the importance of developing healthy sleep-related behaviors during the first months of life. Infant sleep also affects parental sleep, which can affect other outcomes, including maternal sensitivity, relationship quality, parental emotional health, and parenting practices.16,17 Fatigue is related to somatic complaints, anxiety, depression, paranoia, and crashes or unintentional injuries involving motor vehicles among healthy adults39,40 and has been associated with postpartum depression,41 abusive head trauma, and child abuse.42 At a time when family-centered guidance is strongly encouraged, parent fatigue should be considered in any guideline related to infant sleep.
Perhaps our most troubling finding was that room-sharing was associated with overnight transitions to bed-sharing, which is strongly discouraged by the AAP.21 Bed-sharing overnight was more common in our sample among 4- and 9-month-olds who began the night on a separate surface in their parents’ room. This finding is consistent with those from another recently described cohort.43
There are limitations to our analyses. The data come from a randomized controlled trial with 2 interventions. The control group received information aimed at reducing the risk of SIDS, although no recommendation was made regarding sleep location. The RP intervention included guidance to improve sleep duration and sleep-related behaviors, including recommending independent infant sleep by 4 months of age. However, sleep location did not vary significantly by intervention group (data not shown), and all analyses were adjusted for study group. Next, the findings we report are correlational, and causal effects cannot be inferred. It is possible that parents of infants with poorer sleep prefer that infants sleep more closely to them. The relatively homogeneous INSIGHT sample is another potential limitation, as are the self-reported data, although the Brief Infant Sleep Questionnaire has been validated against objective measures of sleep.30 Some previous studies have shown discrepancies between subjective and objective measures of sleep,24,38 and the lack of an objective measure may have resulted in unreported night wakings in which the infant self-soothed back to sleep. This possibility would particularly influence the sleep consolidation data, especially for independent sleepers. However, this possibility, if true, would only reinforce the concept that room-sharing may result in either unnecessary parental responses to infant night wakings or, alternatively, the infant’s expectation of caretaking behaviors from parents, both of which can be expected to lead to decreased sleep for infant and parent. In addition, parental perceptions of their infant’s sleep are the basis for whether they report sleep problems and request guidance from health care providers.
Our data showing that room-sharing families had lower education, income, and less likelihood of living in a single-family residence emphasize that not all parents have a choice regarding their infant’s sleep location and may not have the option to have the infant sleep independently, a factor we could not fully address although demographics and home characteristics were assessed in data analyses. Cultural norms may also lead to preferences for room-sharing in some patient populations, as described by Mileva-Seitz et al.44 Lastly, we did not assess room-sharing beyond 9 months of age.
Conclusions
While substantial progress has been made over the past several decades to improve the safety of infant sleep, the AAP recommendation that parents room-share with their infants until the age of 1 year is not supported by data, is inconsistent with the epidemiology of SIDS, is incongruent with our understanding of socioemotional development in the second half of the first year, and has the potential for unintended consequences for infants and families. Our findings showing poorer sleep-related outcomes and more unsafe sleep practices among dyads who room-share beyond early infancy suggest that the AAP should reconsider and revise the recommendation pending evidence to support room-sharing through the age of 1 year.
Dr Paul co-led all aspects of the study concept, design, and analysis and drafted the initial manuscript; Dr Hohman led the data management, performed the data analyses, and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; Dr Loken contributed to analyses and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; Dr Savage contributed to the design of the study, intervention curriculum development, and data analysis and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; Dr Anzman-Frasca contributed to the design of the study and intervention curriculum development and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; Ms Carper participated in intervention design and delivery and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; Ms Marini contributed to the data collection and analysis and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; Dr Birch co-led all aspects of the study concept, design, and analysis, and participated in critical revision of the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
This trial has been registered at www.clinicaltrials.gov (identifier NCT01167270).
FUNDING: This research was supported by grant R01DK088244 and the Ashley Nicole Shellenberger SIDS Research Fund at the Penn State College of Medicine. Additional support was received from the Children’s Miracle Network at Penn State Children’s Hospital. US Department of Agriculture grant 2011-67001-30117 supported graduate students. REDCap support was received from The Penn State Clinical and Translational Science Institute, Pennsylvania State University’s Clinical and Translational Science Award, and the National Institutes of Health /National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences (NCATS) grant UL1 TR000127. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2017-1323.
References
Competing Interests
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
In this article authors try to assess the association between room-sharing and sleep outcomes of parent and baby. This paper is part of INSIGHT project, a longitudinal, randomized, controlled trial evaluating a responsive parenting intervention designed for the primary prevention of obesity. Considering that in our industrialized societies childhood obesity rates have reached epidemic proportions, is evident that the objectives of the INSIGHT study are of great importance for public health. But, after reading carefully this publication I can only show my surprise and disappointment at the ethical and scientific approval of this project, and the reasons for that are the following:
1. The Sleeping Measurements are Based on the Parents Answers, and although authors admit this limitation, intentionally deny its real importance. There is plenty of evidence that parents who do not sleep with their babies, unlike parents who roomshare, do not report all awakenings or sleep disturbances, and this completely invalidates questionnaires as an acceptable methodology for the studies whose objective is to make official recommendations on infant sleep location. These studies must be focus on what is really happening to infant sleep, not on what parents perceive that is happening.
2. The Responsive Parenting rules that authors give to the parents concerning infant sleep are not responsive at all, because they are not in accordance with infant’s needs, at least not for infants under 6 months, and definitively not for breastfed infants. On the contrary, they are based on a cultural construction, and they ignore completely the natural and healthy nocturnal behavior of breastfed babies.
3. In addition, there is also an enormous cultural bias on the design and interpretation of the study, which prevail over the scientific evidences, in two fundamental areas of infant and children health: infant sleep and breastfeeding. This bias is clearly reflected on the naturalization of solo-sleeping. It is clear from the beginning that not only the authors have big prejudices against cosleeping, but also that they accept as true that the longer and more consolidated night sleep of solo-sleeping babies is healthier than the more fragmented and shorter night sleep of cosleepers, without any real scientific evidence to support it. In fact, scientific evidence has shown us that it is clearly not healthier for breastfed babies.
4. Breastfeeding is not just any feeding, and authors do not take into account differences between breast, formula and complementary feeding when providing advice on solo sleeping, nor when recommending not to feed the baby before or during the night (sleep) after 4 months of age. Breast fed babies (especially exclusive breastfed babies) need to be fed at night, and to forbid it is a big obstacle for breastfeeding. When breastfeeding finds too many obstacles on its way, babies do not gain weight good enough. Usually it forces healthcare professionals to advice mothers to introduce supplementary food too early, or to give to the baby a supplement of formula milk. In this conditions mothers tend to wean too early, what, among other negative effects on baby and mother’s health, also increases the risk of obesity.
Breastfeeding is the gold standard of infant feeding during the first year of life, complemented by solids in the second half year. The promotion of breastfeeding might be an effective measure to prevent obesity (World Health Organization 2014), which was the primary aim of the INSIGHT study. As is well known, breastfeeding reduces mortality and morbidity among infants and mothers. However, insufficient milk supply remains the most frequent reason for the abandonment of breastfeeding (Walker 2016). The most common contributor to insufficient milk supply is mismanagement of breastfeeding, inter alia, a limited number of feedings and scheduled feedings that do not coincide with the infant´s feeding-readiness (Walker 2017). Abandonment of night feedings was found to be associated with weight faltering, return to night feedings (after breastfeeding counselling) led to catch-up growth (Guóth-Gumberger 2011). Also Paul et al. observed that babies sleeping solo breastfeed less at night: They consider it as something positive (with regard to obesity prevention) and do not recognize the danger of weight faltering and premature termination of breastfeeding.
On average, eight to twelve feedings in 24 h are required to sustain exclusive breastfeeding during the first six months (Guóth-Gumberger 2011). In particular, mothers with low breast storage capacity need to breastfeed frequently and, in order to maintain their milk supply, must not skip night feedings (Guóth-Gumberger 2011).
In line with these findings, co-sleeping has been shown to have a strong positive and bidirectional relation to breastfeeding duration (McKenna & Gettler 2016): Breastfeeding mothers co-sleep more than bottle-feeding mothers and mothers who co-sleep breastfeed more frequently and longer than mothers who do not. Even just room-sharing is significantly related to more breastfeeding. Solo-sleeping and too early consolidated night sleep are obstacles to successful breastfeeding.
In our society, we are far away from achieving WHO and AAP recommendations on optimal breastfeeding. This is the reason why it is so important to promote parenting habits that facilitate the initiation and maintenance of breastfeeding. Interventions of the INSIGHT study, which do not distinguish between breastfeeding and formula feeding, might undermine breastfeeding success and thus increase the risk of what they claim to prevent: obesity. These harmful interventions of the INSIGHT study include the promotion of solo sleeping, avoidance of breastfeeding for comfort and for sleep, with 8–12 h (!) without feeding at night beyond the age of four months. Such long gaps without breastfeeding would reduce milk production.
Also, it is most questionable if it is ethical to withhold breastfeeding for comforting a baby, as suggested by the INSIGHT authors for obesity prevention. In many cases, breastfeeding is the most effective method to calm a fussy baby. Accordingly, breastfeeding is an effective non-pharmacological strategy for pain relief, too, more effective than other interventions such as cuddling or topical anesthetic (Harrison et al. 2016).
It is to be hoped that the AAP will ignore the suggestions of the INSIGHT authors and that adverse elements of the INSIGHT interventions will not be incorporated into parenting habits.
Literature:
• Guóth-Gumberger M: Gewichtsverlauf und Stillen. Dokumentieren, Beurteilen, Begleiten. [German; in English: Weight development and Breastfeeding. Recording, Evaluation and Follow-up]. Mabuse, 2011. ISBN 978-3-940529-89-3., p.104.
• Harrison D, Reszel J, Bueno M, Sampson M, Shah VS, Taddio A, Larocque C, Turner L: Breastfeeding for procedural pain in infants beyond the neonatal period. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2016, Issue 10. Art. No.: CD011248.
• McKenna JJ, Gettler LT: There is no such thing as infant sleep, there is no such thing as breastfeeding, there is only breastsleeping. Acta Paediatrica 2016;105 (1):17-21.
• Walker M: Breastfeeding Management for the Clinician, 2016, Jones and Bartlett Publishers, 4. ed., p. 642.
• World Health Organization: Exclusive breastfeeding to reduce the risk of childhood overweight and obesity. Biological, behavioural and contextual rationale. http://www.who.int/elena/titles/bbc/breastfeeding_childhood_obesity/en/, 2014.
This study purports to be a randomized controlled trial of infant-parent room sharing and, in fact, was a randomized controlled trial that sought to modify the sleep habits of parents with young infants through an educational intervention. However, the intervention failed, and the treatment and control groups ended up having the same rate of same-room sleeping with babies. Rather than learning from the failure to evince a change in parental behavior and redesigning a more effective intervention to achieve better compliance (and thus a distinction between treatment arms in sleep habits), the authors instead decided to analyze the differences in outcomes between those who chose to sleep in the same room with their infants and those who chose to sleep apart. First, these two groups looked differently on observable factors--the room sharers were less white, less educated and poorer on average--all factors that may affect child sleep quality through stress and schedule channels that are independent of which room the child is in. Second, the very fact that the intervention did not work to create behavioral differences among the groups suggests that reverse causation is probably a very important factor: Namely, it is the poor sleeping infants who -- despite professional advice -- who end up in their parents' bedrooms. Thus, it is poor sleep rhythms that are the cause, most likely, not the effect, of co-sleeping. It is concerning that the authors even mention their randomized trial, as if it lends causal robustness to a study that is, in fact, plagued by all the endogeneity of any casual correlation in observational data.
I think that this is much more a study of how new parents tend to perceive the amount of sleep that their babies are getting. Self-reported data from surveys of the parents is conjectural at best and influenced by the parent's state of satisfaction with the amount of sleep they and their babies are getting. Obviously a parent would report their baby sleeping more if they were not there in the room to observe each and every waking (except through the filter of a baby monitor or the distance between rooms). And parents who have been sleep deprived for months are clearly going to report better sleep outcomes for their baby after changing their sleeping arrangements, simply because they want their baby to sleep more, and finally have a little bit of relief because she is down the hall! As a new parent, I don't think that this study is really adding much to the discussion of when specifically to move my baby into her room unless you can gather some concrete data about how much the baby is physically sleeping, and not how much the parent is reporting that they are sleeping.