Universal newborn hearing screening has been implemented to detect permanent childhood hearing loss (PCHL) early, with the ultimate goal of improving outcomes through early treatment. However, there is disagreement between studies on the size of this benefit and in some cases whether it is significantly different from 0. There have been no studies of sufficient size in which researchers have determined reliably whether the effect varies with degree of PCHL. We aimed to explore how intervention timing influences 5-year language in children with PCHL.
Via a prospective study of 350 children, we used standard multiple regression analyses to investigate the effect of age at intervention or hearing screening on language outcomes after allowing for the effects of nonverbal IQ, degree of PCHL, sex, birth weight, maternal education, additional disabilities, and communication mode.
The benefit of early intervention for language development increased as hearing loss increased. Children whose amplification started at age 24 months had poorer language than those whose amplification started at 3 months. The difference was larger for 70-dB HL (−11.8 score points; 95% confidence interval [95% CI]: −18.7 to −4.8) than for 50-dB HL (−6.8; 95% CI: −10.8 to −2.8). Children who received cochlear implants at 24 months had poorer language than those implanted at 6 months (−21.4; 95% CI: −33.8 to −9.0). There was no significant effect of screening on outcomes.
Early intervention improves language outcomes, thereby lending support to streamlining clinical pathways to ensure early amplification and cochlear implantation after diagnosis.
Universal newborn hearing screening is associated with early detection of permanent childhood hearing loss and superior language outcomes. However, the effect of variation of age at intervention and in severity of hearing loss on these associations is unknown.
Compared with the same intervention at 24 months, 5-year language scores were 0.8 and 0.55 SD higher in children with 70- and 50-dB hearing loss, respectively, with amplification at 3 months and 1.4 SD higher with implantation at 6 months.
Bilateral permanent childhood hearing loss (PCHL), which occurs in 1 to 2 per 1000 newborns,1,2 has major negative impacts on children’s development,3,–5 thereby incurring high societal costs.6 Therefore, several program-based studies7,–9 in which researchers showed a retrospective association between earlier identification and better preschool language, despite methodological limitations,10 have driven widespread implementation of universal newborn hearing screening (UNHS). Although the 2001 US Preventive Services Task Force systematic review found good evidence that UNHS leads to earlier identification and treatment, it concluded that “evidence to determine whether earlier treatment resulting from screening leads to clinically important improvement in speech and language is … inconclusive.”10
Subsequent to the review, in several population-based studies, researchers have examined the relationship of screening or early intervention with child outcomes. In the Wessex quasi-experimental study, Kennedy et al11 compared the language of 7- to 8-year-old English children with PCHL who were born in the mid-1990s in areas with and without UNHS. Exposure to UNHS had a significant positive effect on receptive language but not expressive language or speech production. Confirmation of PCHL by age 9 months (comprising 67% screened and 27% unscreened children) was associated with higher scores for receptive and expressive language but not for speech production. Because the study was conducted before the modernization of English audiological services, and confirmation by age 9 months would be considered “late” relative to current recommendations,12 the impact of early intervention remains to be further investigated.
In contrast, Wake et al13 reported on a community-based cohort of children with PCHL who were exposed to risk-based newborn screening in Australia and found no significant relationship between age at diagnosis and speech and language outcomes at 7 to 8 years old. The small number of children diagnosed with PCHL before 6 months of age (n = 11) may provide inadequate power to evaluate an age effect. In a Canadian study,14 researchers compared language abilities between screened and unscreened children aged 2 to 5 years and did not find significant differences between groups. The finding may have been limited by the small sample size (n = 26 screened, 39 not screened).
In the Netherlands, Korver et al15 compared language scores of Dutch children aged 3 to 5 years born in UNHS and non-UNHS regions and found no significant difference between groups. They did not adjust for hearing loss severity, which has been negatively associated with language outcomes in previous studies,13,16 and relied on parent reports without direct assessments of children’s abilities. The effect of early confirmation of PCHL on language outcomes was not examined.
The 2008 update of the US Preventive Services Task Force review17 and a recent review by Pimperton and Kennedy18 confirmed that there was good evidence that UNHS leads to earlier intervention, defined by cutoff points at 67 and 9 months,11 and improves language outcomes. The benefit of UNHS was smaller than that of early identification, presumably because of the failure of screening programs to screen the entire target population or to proceed promptly to identification of true cases among children screening positive for PCHL.18
In a recent prospective study, the Longitudinal Outcomes of Children with Hearing Impairment (LOCHI) study, researchers evaluated the effect of age at intervention on outcomes of a population-based cohort born between 2002 and 2007 in the states of New South Wales, Victoria, and Queensland in Australia. All children with PCHL receive uniform postdiagnostic services administered by a national government-funded organization (Australian Hearing [AH]) at no cost to families, but access to UNHS depended on whether state-funded UNHS was operating.19 Through the service network, the researchers for the LOCHI study enrolled children whose PCHL was identified through either UNHS or standard care. On average, better language at 3 years of age was significantly associated with earlier fitting of a cochlear implant (CI), but age at the fitting of a hearing aid (HA) had a weak effect on outcomes.16 Possibly, assessments at this young age might not provide sufficient scope for manifestation of the benefits of early intervention on language development.14,20
Although researchers who in previous studies have included sufficient observations have reported some benefits of early intervention for later language skills, their studies have not been powered to determine if the extent of the benefit varies with severity of PCHL. Here, we addressed this question by drawing on the 5-year language outcomes of the cohort in the LOCHI study.
We aimed to explore the following:
effects of age at HA fitting by degree of hearing loss for children using HAs; and
effects of age at CI activation for children using CIs.
We hypothesized that children who started amplification earlier would have better language, and the benefit would increase with hearing loss. Also, we hypothesized that children who received a CI earlier would have better language.
Our secondary aim was to examine the impact of UNHS on outcomes separately for children using HAs or CIs. We hypothesized that because access to UNHS decreases age at intervention (fitting of HA or CI), access to UNHS would improve outcomes.
Methods
Study Population
The participants were 350 children with PCHL and a comparison group of 120 children with normal hearing that was similar to the group of children with PCHL with respect to age, sex, and socioeconomic status (SES). In this study, we focused on the impact of intervention within the age range for which there is uncertainty about the effect of age of intervention, and we therefore excluded children whose device was fitted later than 3 years of age and those whose long-term audiograms revealed a progressive hearing loss. The participants included children from the population-based cohort from 3 states in Australia who participated in the LOCHI study (see Introduction and Fig 1) and who had received by 3 years of age a device of the same type (ie, HA or CI) that they were using at age 5 years. This study was approved by the AH Human Research Ethics Committee.
Sampling
The AH national service network served as the sampling frame. All families of children who met inclusion criteria were presented with an information statement and consent form at a time as close to initial fitting of HAs as possible, in person or by post. Once written informed consent was obtained from parents, their child was assessed at several intervals.
Postdiagnostic follow-up for all children with PCHL, including this cohort, is performed by AH audiologists until the children reach 26 years of age. The service includes audiological assessment and fitting of HAs and referrals for CI, medical, and communication intervention, in accordance with the AH national protocol.21
Measures
Main outcome measures22,–32 are summarized in Table 1. Standardized measures of language, vocabulary, letter knowledge, speech production, and oral reading were directly administered to children at age 5 to 6 years by speech pathologists who were blinded, as much as possible, to age at intervention and hearing loss severity. Parents reported on their child’s functional auditory performance in real life, language, and social skills. All measures are widely used for assessing development of children with normal hearing or PCHL, with known validity and reliability. The same test instruments and protocols were administered across all sites.
Measures
. | Administration . | Measure . | Information About Measure and Scoring . |
---|---|---|---|
Receptive language | Direct assessment | Preschool Language Scale version 422 | Auditory comprehension subscale. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 50–140. |
Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Language comprehension subscale, based on 50 items. Quotients calculated. | |
Expressive language | Direct assessment | Preschool Language Scale version 422 | Expressive communication subscale. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 50–140. |
Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Expressive language subscale, based on 50 items. Quotients. | |
Receptive vocabulary | Direct assessment | Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test version 424 | 228 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 20–160. |
Expressive vocabulary | Direct assessment | Expressive Vocabulary Test version 225 | 190 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 20–160. |
Reading | Direct assessment | Woodcock diagnostic reading battery26 | 3 subscale scores: word attack, word identification, and passage comprehension. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Mathematics | Direct assessment | Wechsler individual achievement test27 | Mathematics reasoning subtest. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Number | Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Number subscale, based on rating of 15 items. Quotients. |
Letter knowledge | Direct assessment | Phonological abilities test28 | Letter knowledge subtest, measures letter name. Raw score range 0–26. |
Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Letter subscale, based on rating of 15 items. Quotients. | |
Speech production | Direct assessment | Diagnostic evaluation of articulation and phonology29 | 2 subscale scores: vowel and consonant, each based on 50 target words elicited by pictures. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Functional auditory behavior | Parent report | Parents’ Evaluation of Aural/Oral Performance of Children30 | 11 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Social skills | Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Social subscale, based on ratings of 40 items. Quotients. |
Behavior problems | Parent report | Strengths and difficulties questionnaire31 | 25 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Teacher report | Strengths and difficulties questionnaire31 | 25 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. | |
Health-related quality of life | Parent report | Pediatric quality of life inventory32 | 23-items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
. | Administration . | Measure . | Information About Measure and Scoring . |
---|---|---|---|
Receptive language | Direct assessment | Preschool Language Scale version 422 | Auditory comprehension subscale. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 50–140. |
Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Language comprehension subscale, based on 50 items. Quotients calculated. | |
Expressive language | Direct assessment | Preschool Language Scale version 422 | Expressive communication subscale. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 50–140. |
Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Expressive language subscale, based on 50 items. Quotients. | |
Receptive vocabulary | Direct assessment | Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test version 424 | 228 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 20–160. |
Expressive vocabulary | Direct assessment | Expressive Vocabulary Test version 225 | 190 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15, range 20–160. |
Reading | Direct assessment | Woodcock diagnostic reading battery26 | 3 subscale scores: word attack, word identification, and passage comprehension. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Mathematics | Direct assessment | Wechsler individual achievement test27 | Mathematics reasoning subtest. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Number | Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Number subscale, based on rating of 15 items. Quotients. |
Letter knowledge | Direct assessment | Phonological abilities test28 | Letter knowledge subtest, measures letter name. Raw score range 0–26. |
Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Letter subscale, based on rating of 15 items. Quotients. | |
Speech production | Direct assessment | Diagnostic evaluation of articulation and phonology29 | 2 subscale scores: vowel and consonant, each based on 50 target words elicited by pictures. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Functional auditory behavior | Parent report | Parents’ Evaluation of Aural/Oral Performance of Children30 | 11 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Social skills | Parent report | Child development inventory23 | Social subscale, based on ratings of 40 items. Quotients. |
Behavior problems | Parent report | Strengths and difficulties questionnaire31 | 25 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Teacher report | Strengths and difficulties questionnaire31 | 25 items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. | |
Health-related quality of life | Parent report | Pediatric quality of life inventory32 | 23-items. Standardized: mean 100, SD 15. |
Information about age at HA fitting, age at CI activation, audiometric thresholds, and hearing device were obtained from clinical records. The degree of PCHL at the time of evaluation was characterized in terms of better-ear 4-frequency average (BE4FA) hearing loss (average of hearing thresholds at 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz) in dB hearing level (HL). Nonverbal IQ was assessed by a psychologist. Parents completed custom-designed questionnaires on demographic characteristics.
Potential a priori confounders were determined on the basis of current literature, including earlier findings at 3 years of age.16 The variables included birth weight, sex, degree of hearing loss, nonverbal IQ, additional disabilities, maternal education (university degree, vocational training certificate, or ≤12 years of formal schooling), SES (census-based Index of Relative Socioeconomic Advantage and Disadvantage for home postcode33; higher scores reflect greater advantage), and communication mode during educational intervention (oral versus combined [speech and sign]). These were used as predictors in regression analyses to maximally account for variance in scores so as to increase the sensitivity with which we can assess the impact of intervention timing or access to UNHS.
Statistical Analysis
The statistical analysis was done by using R, version 3.0.2,34 and Statistica, version 10.35 All analyses used 2-tailed tests unless otherwise stated, with statistical significance set at P < .05.
Children unable to be directly assessed because of parent-reported or apparent disabilities or not using spoken English were assigned the poorest score observed for that test (receptive and expressive language: n = 33, 9.4%; vocabulary: n = 62, 17.7%; speech production: n = 47, 13.4%; reading: n = 66, 18.9%; letter name: n = 63, 18.0%; mathematics: n = 69, 19.7%). A multiple imputation method36 was applied to handle missing values in variables, assuming that data were missing at random. The analysis was averaged over 10 imputations.
Scores from 20 measures were aggregated by using factor analysis to form a global language score. This approach was justified by an exploratory factor analysis showing that such measures all loaded onto a single underlying factor. The global score was scaled so that a normal population would have a mean of 100 and an SD of 15. The use of a global score eliminates the disadvantages associated with performing multiple hypothesis testing on correlated data and increases reliability by reducing random measurement error through averaging across measures.
Outcomes by Age at HA Fitting and Hearing Loss Severity for the HA Group (Aim 1)
We performed standard multiple linear regression analysis with the global score as the dependent variable. Predictors included age at initial HA fitting (log transformed), degree of PCHL, nonverbal IQ, birth weight, and an interaction between age at HA fitting and degree of PCHL as continuous variables and sex, additional disabilities, maternal education, SES, and communication mode as categorical variables.
Outcomes by Age at Cochlear Implantation for the CI Group (Aim 2)
We performed a multiple linear regression analysis using the global score as a dependent variable, with predictor variables as for the HA group, but we performed it with age at HA fitting replaced by age at activation of the first CI (log transformed) and without the interaction between age at HA fitting and degree of PCHL.
Outcomes by Screening Status for the HA and CI Groups (Aim 3)
Analyses for subgroups of users of HAs and CIs were as described above but with age at intervention replaced by screening status (screened vs nonscreened).
Results
Figure 1 summarizes participant enrollment. Of the 728 infants who met inclusion criteria for the LOCHI study, 545 (74.8%) families gave consent for participation. Participants were similar to nonparticipants with respect to age, sex, degree of PCHL, and SES. By 5 years of age, 472 were enrolled in the study, of whom 384 children had nonprogressive hearing loss. Of these, 350 completed the required evaluations. All except 3 completed at least 1 directly administered test. Demographic characteristics of the cohort are shown in Table 2.
Characteristics of the Sample
. | HA at 5 ya: Screened N = 174 . | HA at 5 y: Non-screened N = 47 . | Difference Pb . | CI at 5 y: aScreened N = 84 . | CI at 5 y: Nonscreened N = 20 . | Difference P . | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Child | Age at HA fitting in months, mean (SD) | 9.1 (8.9) | 19.1 (10.7) | <.001 | 4.0 (4.2) | 12.9 (6.3) | <.001 |
Age at HA fitting in months, median (IQR)c | 5.1 (2.6–12.2) | 21.3 (9.2–28.6) | — | 2.5 (1.8 – 4.1) | 12.8 (7.4 –18.2) | — | |
Fitted with HAs before 6 mo of age, n (%)d | 97 (55.7%) | 9 (19.1%) | <.001 | 70 (83.3%) | 3 (15.0%) | <.001 | |
Age at activation of first CI in months, median (IQR) | — | — | — | 13.04 (9.7 –19.2) | 20.9 (16.3 –26.1) | — | |
Fitted with CI before 12 mo of age, n (%) | — | — | — | 37 (44.0%) | 2 (10.0%) | .01 | |
Male sex, n (%) | 97 (55.7%) | 30 (63.8%) | .41 | 41 (46.5%) | 5 (25.0%) | .09 | |
Birth weight (g), mean (SD) | 2967.7 (1022.2) | 3144.2 (843.4) | .67 | 3184.8 (883.5) | 2923.4 (941.1) | .18 | |
Degree of hearing loss depicted by BE4FA hearing thresholds in dB HL, mean (SD) | 49.1 (14.8) | 47.0 (15.9) | .42 | 101.9 (14.5) | 97.0 (16.9) | .29 | |
Mild (BE4FA ≤ 40 dB HL), n (%) | 47 (27.0%) | 13 (27.7%) | — | — | — | — | |
Moderate (BE4FA: 41–60 dBHL), n (%) | 88 (50.6%) | 26 (55.3%) | — | — | — | — | |
Severe (BE4FA: 61–80 dB HL), n (%) | 36 (20.7%) | 7 (14.9%) | — | 7 (8.6%) | 3 (20%) | — | |
Profound (BE4FA: >80 dB HL), n (%) | 3 (1.7%) | 1 (2.1%) | — | 74 (91.4%) | 12 (80%) | — | |
Presence of any additional disabilities, n (%) | 49 (31.3%) | 21 (47.7%) | .06 | 23 (31.9%) | 2 (11.1%) | .14 | |
Cerebral palsy | 10 (6.3%) | 3 (6.7%) | — | 5 (6.7%) | 2 (11.1%) | — | |
Autism spectrum disorder | 6 (2.8%) | 1 (2.3%) | — | 4 (5.4%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Vision impairment | 30 (13.9%) | 8 (17.8%) | — | 4 (5.4%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Cranio-facial abnormalities | 9 (5.7%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | 1 (1.3%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Genetic | 17 (10.6%) | 4 (8.9%) | — | 5 (6.8%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Other | 20 (12.6%) | 12 (26.7%) | — | 10 (13.5%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Nonverbal cognitive ability, mean (SD) | 95.7 (25.4) | 96.3 (24.5) | .81 | 92.5 (26.4) | 92.1 (24.0) | .74 | |
Oral communication during educational intervention, n (%) | 79 (83.2%) | 19 (76.0%) | .59 | 44 (83.0%) | 5 (62.5%) | .38 | |
Family | Language other than English household, n (%) | 33 (21.4%) | 6 (14.6%) | .46 | 14 (20.6%) | 3 (20.0%) | .99 |
SESe, mean (SD) | 1018.8 (74.5) | 1023.9 (74.8) | .68 | 1015.6 (73.5) | 1010.5 (78.0) | .83 | |
Maternal education: ≤12 y schooling, n (%) | 59 (34.7%) | 19 (43.2%) | .39 | 24 (29.6%) | 8 (42.1%) | .44 | |
Maternal education: vocational training certificate, n (%) | 43 (25.3%) | 10 (22.7%) | .88 | 18 (22.2%) | 5 (26.3%) | .94 | |
Maternal education: university degree, n (%) | 68 (40.0%) | 15 (34.1%) | .59 | 39 (48.1%) | 6 (31.6%) | .29 |
. | HA at 5 ya: Screened N = 174 . | HA at 5 y: Non-screened N = 47 . | Difference Pb . | CI at 5 y: aScreened N = 84 . | CI at 5 y: Nonscreened N = 20 . | Difference P . | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Child | Age at HA fitting in months, mean (SD) | 9.1 (8.9) | 19.1 (10.7) | <.001 | 4.0 (4.2) | 12.9 (6.3) | <.001 |
Age at HA fitting in months, median (IQR)c | 5.1 (2.6–12.2) | 21.3 (9.2–28.6) | — | 2.5 (1.8 – 4.1) | 12.8 (7.4 –18.2) | — | |
Fitted with HAs before 6 mo of age, n (%)d | 97 (55.7%) | 9 (19.1%) | <.001 | 70 (83.3%) | 3 (15.0%) | <.001 | |
Age at activation of first CI in months, median (IQR) | — | — | — | 13.04 (9.7 –19.2) | 20.9 (16.3 –26.1) | — | |
Fitted with CI before 12 mo of age, n (%) | — | — | — | 37 (44.0%) | 2 (10.0%) | .01 | |
Male sex, n (%) | 97 (55.7%) | 30 (63.8%) | .41 | 41 (46.5%) | 5 (25.0%) | .09 | |
Birth weight (g), mean (SD) | 2967.7 (1022.2) | 3144.2 (843.4) | .67 | 3184.8 (883.5) | 2923.4 (941.1) | .18 | |
Degree of hearing loss depicted by BE4FA hearing thresholds in dB HL, mean (SD) | 49.1 (14.8) | 47.0 (15.9) | .42 | 101.9 (14.5) | 97.0 (16.9) | .29 | |
Mild (BE4FA ≤ 40 dB HL), n (%) | 47 (27.0%) | 13 (27.7%) | — | — | — | — | |
Moderate (BE4FA: 41–60 dBHL), n (%) | 88 (50.6%) | 26 (55.3%) | — | — | — | — | |
Severe (BE4FA: 61–80 dB HL), n (%) | 36 (20.7%) | 7 (14.9%) | — | 7 (8.6%) | 3 (20%) | — | |
Profound (BE4FA: >80 dB HL), n (%) | 3 (1.7%) | 1 (2.1%) | — | 74 (91.4%) | 12 (80%) | — | |
Presence of any additional disabilities, n (%) | 49 (31.3%) | 21 (47.7%) | .06 | 23 (31.9%) | 2 (11.1%) | .14 | |
Cerebral palsy | 10 (6.3%) | 3 (6.7%) | — | 5 (6.7%) | 2 (11.1%) | — | |
Autism spectrum disorder | 6 (2.8%) | 1 (2.3%) | — | 4 (5.4%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Vision impairment | 30 (13.9%) | 8 (17.8%) | — | 4 (5.4%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Cranio-facial abnormalities | 9 (5.7%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | 1 (1.3%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Genetic | 17 (10.6%) | 4 (8.9%) | — | 5 (6.8%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Other | 20 (12.6%) | 12 (26.7%) | — | 10 (13.5%) | 0 (0.0%) | — | |
Nonverbal cognitive ability, mean (SD) | 95.7 (25.4) | 96.3 (24.5) | .81 | 92.5 (26.4) | 92.1 (24.0) | .74 | |
Oral communication during educational intervention, n (%) | 79 (83.2%) | 19 (76.0%) | .59 | 44 (83.0%) | 5 (62.5%) | .38 | |
Family | Language other than English household, n (%) | 33 (21.4%) | 6 (14.6%) | .46 | 14 (20.6%) | 3 (20.0%) | .99 |
SESe, mean (SD) | 1018.8 (74.5) | 1023.9 (74.8) | .68 | 1015.6 (73.5) | 1010.5 (78.0) | .83 | |
Maternal education: ≤12 y schooling, n (%) | 59 (34.7%) | 19 (43.2%) | .39 | 24 (29.6%) | 8 (42.1%) | .44 | |
Maternal education: vocational training certificate, n (%) | 43 (25.3%) | 10 (22.7%) | .88 | 18 (22.2%) | 5 (26.3%) | .94 | |
Maternal education: university degree, n (%) | 68 (40.0%) | 15 (34.1%) | .59 | 39 (48.1%) | 6 (31.6%) | .29 |
IQR, interquartile range; —, not applicable.
Accessed UNHS. Information about screening status was missing for 25 of the 350 children.
P value for the test of whether the distributions in the screened and nonscreened groups are the same using Mann–Whitney U tests for the continuous variables and tests of proportions for the discrete variables.
Median age at HA fitting of the entire screened group comprising users of HA and CI was 3.9 mo (IQR: 2.3–10.1), and the median age at fitting of the entire nonscreened group was 17.3 mo (IQR: 7.5–25.9).
All percentages are relative to the number of nonmissing values.
SES represented by the Index of Relative Social Advantage and Disadvantage, which has a mean of 1000 and an SD of 100. The study cohort has a range of 300–1250.
Figure 2 shows the distribution of the global language scores of the cohort.
Distribution of global language scores. The dashed line depicts the theoretical distribution of scores in the “normal” population (normally distributed with mean = 100, SD = 15).
Distribution of global language scores. The dashed line depicts the theoretical distribution of scores in the “normal” population (normally distributed with mean = 100, SD = 15).
Outcomes by Age at HA Fitting and Hearing Loss Severity for the HA Group (Aim 1)
Children who received HAs earlier had better language outcomes. In addition, the regression model indicates that the effect of age at intervention increased as hearing loss increased (P = .035; 1-tailed). Table 3 gives the effect sizes associated with changes in age at amplification for different degrees of PCHL. Compared with amplification at 3 months, those who started amplification at 24 months had poorer language: −11.8 score points (95% CI: −18.7 to −4.8) for 70-dB HL and −6.8 score points (95% CI: −10.8 to −2.8) for 50-dB HL (see Fig 3). These effects were observed after adjustments for nonverbal IQ, birth weight, additional disabilities, maternal education, sex, SES, and communication mode in a model that accounted for 74% of total variance in global score.
Group With HAs
Age at Amplification . | Estimate and 95% CI . | ||
---|---|---|---|
BE4FA = 30 dB HL | BE4FA = 50 dB HL | BE4FA = 70 dB HL | |
3 → 6 | −0.7 (−4.3 to 2.9) | −2.3 (−5.4 to 0.8) | −4.0 (−7.6 to −0.4) |
6 → 12 | −0.6 (−2.8 to 1.6) | −2.3 (−3.6 to −0.9) | −3.9 (−6.3 to −1.6) |
12 → 18 | −0.3 (−2.3 to 1.6) | −1.3 (−3.0 to 0.4) | −2.3 (−4.3 to −0.2) |
18 → 24 | −0.2 (−1.8 to 1.4) | −0.9 (−2.4 to 0.6) | −1.6 (−3.3 to 0.1) |
Age at Amplification . | Estimate and 95% CI . | ||
---|---|---|---|
BE4FA = 30 dB HL | BE4FA = 50 dB HL | BE4FA = 70 dB HL | |
3 → 6 | −0.7 (−4.3 to 2.9) | −2.3 (−5.4 to 0.8) | −4.0 (−7.6 to −0.4) |
6 → 12 | −0.6 (−2.8 to 1.6) | −2.3 (−3.6 to −0.9) | −3.9 (−6.3 to −1.6) |
12 → 18 | −0.3 (−2.3 to 1.6) | −1.3 (−3.0 to 0.4) | −2.3 (−4.3 to −0.2) |
18 → 24 | −0.2 (−1.8 to 1.4) | −0.9 (−2.4 to 0.6) | −1.6 (−3.3 to 0.1) |
The estimated effect size and 95% CI in the mean global language score associated with the stated change in the age at intervention (eg, “compared with fitting at 3 mo, fitting at 6 mo” is expressed as “3 → 6”), if the other predictor variables are constant. The degree of hearing loss is expressed in terms of BE4FA in dB HL.
Adjusted global language scores by age at HA fitting (log transformed) in children using HAs at age 5 years. For display, children are grouped according to their BE4FA hearing loss in terms of dB HL. Data points are included for children who had 20-dB HL < BE4FA hearing loss < 40-dB HL in the first panel, 40-dB HL < BE4FA hearing loss < 60-dB HL in the second panel, and 60-dB HL < BE4FA hearing loss < 80-dB HL in the third panel. The points are adjusted with nonverbal IQ, birth weight, and SES set to the mean values of the entire HA group, no additional disabilities, and with maternal education, sex, and communication mode set to university degree, girl, and oral, respectively. In each panel, the regression line shows predicted mean score, and the shaded band depicts the 95% CI.
Adjusted global language scores by age at HA fitting (log transformed) in children using HAs at age 5 years. For display, children are grouped according to their BE4FA hearing loss in terms of dB HL. Data points are included for children who had 20-dB HL < BE4FA hearing loss < 40-dB HL in the first panel, 40-dB HL < BE4FA hearing loss < 60-dB HL in the second panel, and 60-dB HL < BE4FA hearing loss < 80-dB HL in the third panel. The points are adjusted with nonverbal IQ, birth weight, and SES set to the mean values of the entire HA group, no additional disabilities, and with maternal education, sex, and communication mode set to university degree, girl, and oral, respectively. In each panel, the regression line shows predicted mean score, and the shaded band depicts the 95% CI.
Outcomes by Age at CI (Aim 2)
Earlier age at CI activation was associated with better language at 5 years of age. Table 4 gives the effect sizes for variations in age at implantation. Compared with those who first received CIs at age 6 months, children who received a CI at 24 months had lower scores by 1.4 SD (−21.4 score points, 95% CI: −33.8 to −9.0). These effects were observed after adjustments for nonverbal IQ, additional disabilities, maternal education, and communication mode in a multilinear regression model that accounted for 70% of total variance in global score. Figure 4 shows the relationship between age at implantation and the adjusted global scores.
Group With CIs
Age at CI Activation . | Estimate and 95% CI . |
---|---|
6 → 12 | −10.8 (−22.3 to 0.8) |
12 → 18 | −6.2 (−9.3 to −3.1) |
18 → 24 | −4.4 (−8.6 to −0.1) |
Age at CI Activation . | Estimate and 95% CI . |
---|---|
6 → 12 | −10.8 (−22.3 to 0.8) |
12 → 18 | −6.2 (−9.3 to −3.1) |
18 → 24 | −4.4 (−8.6 to −0.1) |
The estimated effect size and 95% CI in the mean global language score associated with the stated change in the age at intervention (eg, “compared with CI activation at 6 mo, activation at 12 mo” is expressed as “6 → 12”), if the other predictor variables are constant.
Adjusted global language scores by age at CI activation (log transformed). The regression line shows predicted mean score, and the shaded band depicts the 95% CI.
Adjusted global language scores by age at CI activation (log transformed). The regression line shows predicted mean score, and the shaded band depicts the 95% CI.
Outcomes by Screening Status (Aim 3)
The effect size of hearing screening (screened vs nonscreened) was 0.3 score points (95% CI: −4.2 to 4.9; P = .88) for children using HAs and 6.4 score points (95% CI: −2.8 to 16.2; P = .19) for children using CIs.
Discussion
Principal Findings
In this prospective study, we demonstrated a strong positive effect of early intervention on 5-year language in children with PCHL. The younger a child received intervention, the better the language outcomes. In addition, more substantial benefits of earlier access to useful hearing via both HAs and CIs were obtained by those with worse hearing. Earlier intervention, rather than access to UNHS, improved outcomes.
Strengths of the Study
This study has a sample size that was powered for investigating the effect of intervention timing on outcomes separately for children using HAs or CIs after allowing for the effects of multiple demographic characteristics. In this relatively large study, we include language data from 350 children fitted with HAs before 3 years of age, comprising 189 (54%) before age 6 months (at 5 years: 111 use HAs, 78 use CIs) and 161 between 6 months and 3 years (at 5 years: 125 use HAs, 36 use CIs). These data therefore constitute the best opportunity to examine how severity of PCHL influences the association between age at intervention and language outcomes. Because all children received uniform hearing services and high-level technology from a single government-funded agency, we controlled for variations in postdiagnostic services. We evaluated outcomes using direct assessments of children by researchers who were blinded to age at diagnosis and parent reports.
The median age at HA fitting for the entire screened group (3.9 months; interquartile range: 2.3–10.1) is earlier than that of the cohorts in the Wessex study (15 months; interquartile range: 10–40),11 the Netherlands study (mean 15.7 months; SD, 14.0),15 and an earlier Australian study.37 Unlike recent population-based studies in which researchers included few children with CIs (Wessex study: 16; Netherlands study: 32),11,15 the present sample comprised 114 children who received CIs before 3 years of age, 42 (36.5%) of whom received them before 12 months of age. All children were assessed at 5 years of age, unlike previous studies that evaluated children at an older (Wessex study: 6–10 years) or a younger age (Netherlands study: 3–5 years).
Limitations
Although our sample size was large, the 48.1% (350 out of 728) inclusion rate was lower than that in the Wessex study,11 but it was comparable to the proportion with measured outcomes in the Netherlands study15 or a recent Australian study.38 As age, sex, hearing loss and SES were similar in participants and nonparticipants, response bias seems unlikely. The participation rate may limit inferences on the merits of UNHS, but it does not affect conclusions on the impact of intervention timing on outcomes.
Although our regression models have accounted for a major proportion of variance in global scores, there remains about 30% of unexplained variance, so some combination of other factors and measurement error has also influenced outcomes. Future investigations of the effect of intervention timing on scores for specific assessments would also be worthwhile because age at intervention may affect some abilities more than others.18
Finally, we excluded children with progressive hearing loss (n = 88), so the results will likely not generalize to that population.
Interpretation in Light of Other Studies
With our findings, we broadly agree with but also extend findings in recent reports. Earlier intervention improved language outcomes of children with PCHL.18 Unlike previous studies in which researchers used cutoff points for early identification of 67 or 9 months,11 suggesting a time window for language development to maximally benefit from intervention or a “sensitive period” that ends before 1 year,18 our data do not support this concept. Rather, the earlier intervention is provided (at least down to 3 months for HA and 6 months for CI) over the first 3 years of life, the better. Furthermore, the benefit increases as hearing loss increases (see Tables 3 and 4). The positive strong effect of early CI is consistent with results at age 3 years16 and previous literature.39,40
Although access to UNHS led to earlier intervention (72% of the screened group commenced amplification before 6 months, compared with 32% in the unscreened group), and earlier intervention led to better outcomes, there is insufficient evidence to conclude that UNHS is beneficial. The diminished effect size of UNHS compared with that of age at intervention is likely because although UNHS maximizes the opportunity of early intervention, not all screened children commenced amplification early and not all unscreened children commenced amplification late (see Table 2).
The present findings, consistent with a recent review of evidence,18 have important implications. First, timely device-fitting and educational support needs to follow UNHS to ensure earlier access to hearing, communication, and language development.41 Second, research needs to focus on postdiagnostic intervention to capitalize on early detection. Third, future follow-up of the existing cohort could evaluate whether the long-term language outcomes improve as the age at intervention decreases.42
Conclusions
In this study, we show that it is vital to implement a seamless clinical pathway from screening to diagnosis to intervention (fitting HA or CI) so that the opportunity offered by UNHS to improve language development can be captured. In addition, the significance of early implantation highlights the importance of vigilant monitoring of early outcomes with amplification so that children at risk for language delay can receive CIs at the earliest possible age.
Dr Ching conceptualized and designed the study, took overall responsibility for all aspects of the study, drafted the initial manuscript, and revised the manuscript for submission; Dr Dillon consulted in the design of the study, guided the analysis of data and interpretation of data, and reviewed the manuscript; Ms Button was responsible for acquisition of the evaluation data, provided guidance regarding data collection, and reviewed the manuscript; Mr Seeto was responsible for the analysis of data, in consultation with Drs Dillon and Ching, and reviewed the manuscript; Ms Van Buynder was responsible for acquisition of the audiological data, provided guidance regarding data collection, and reviewed the manuscript; Ms Marnane assisted with management of the project, coordinated acquisition of demographic data and collation of data, and reviewed the manuscript; Drs Cupples and Leigh consulted in the design of the study, provided guidance regarding the data collection instruments, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.
FUNDING: The project described was partly supported by Award R01DC008080 from the National Institute on Deafness and Other Communication Disorders. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute On Deafness And Other Communication Disorders or the National Institutes of Health. The project was also supported by the Commonwealth of Australia through the Office of Hearing Services and through the establishment of the HEARing Cooperative Research Center and the Cooperative Research Centres Programme. The funding organisations had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).
Acknowledgments
We thank all the families and children for their participation. We also acknowledge the assistance of the many clinicians who provided audiological management of the children and the assistance of professionals at early intervention agencies and CI clinics in the collection of demographic and outcomes data. We also thank Julia Day, Kathryn Crowe, and Nicole Mahler for their assistance with data collection in the early phase of the study and Vicky W. Zhang and Sanna Hou for their help with preparation of the manuscript. Last but not least, we thank the anonymous reviewers for their detailed comments and suggestions for improving earlier versions of the manuscript.
References
Competing Interests
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.