Although prenatal alcohol and nicotine exposure are associated with reduced cognition in children, associations between consumption of alcohol during lactation and cognition have not been examined. We aimed to examine whether drinking or smoking while breastfeeding lowers children’s cognitive scores. We hypothesized that increased drinking or smoking would be associated with dose-dependent cognitive reductions.
Data were sourced from Growing Up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Participants were 5107 Australian infants recruited in 2004 and assessed every 2 years. Multivariable linear regression analyses assessed relationships between drinking and smoking habits of breastfeeding mothers and children’s Matrix Reasoning, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition and Who Am I? scores at later waves.
Increased or riskier wave 1 maternal alcohol consumption was associated with reductions in Matrix Reasoning scores at age 6 to 7 years in children who had been breastfed (B = −0.11; SE = 0.03; 95% confidence interval: −0.18 to −0.04; P = .01). This relationship was not evident in infants who had never breastfed (B = −0.02; SE = 0.10; 95% confidence interval = −0.20 to 0.17; P = .87). Smoking during lactation was not associated with any outcome variable.
Exposing infants to alcohol through breastmilk may cause dose-dependent reductions in their cognitive abilities. This reduction was observed at age 6 to 7 years but was not sustained at age 10 to 11 years. Although the relationship is small, it may be clinically significant when mothers consume alcohol regularly or binge drink. Further analyses will assess relationships between alcohol consumption or tobacco smoking during lactation and academic, developmental, physical, and behavioral outcomes in children.
Although alcohol is a known teratogen, studies of maternal alcohol use during breastfeeding and infants’ basic developmental scores have produced mixed results. No previous study has assessed the impact of maternal drinking or smoking on cognitive outcomes in the child.
This is the first study to directly examine cognitive outcomes in relation to lactational alcohol and nicotine exposure. Increased or riskier maternal alcohol consumption while breastfeeding was associated with reduced abstract reasoning ability in the child at age 6 to 7 years.
Although teratogenic effects of alcohol are well documented,1,–4 cognitive risks of alcohol and breastfeeding are unknown.5 Likewise, although tobacco smoking during pregnancy is associated with reductions in childhood cognition,6 cognitive effects from smoking tobacco during lactation have not been researched. Because alcohol7 and nicotine8 are available in breastmilk after maternal intake, understanding whether smoking tobacco or drinking alcohol during lactation impacts children’s cognitive abilities is important.
Although the World Health Organization recommends avoiding alcohol and drugs while breastfeeding,9 12% to 83%5,10,–16 of breastfeeding women report consuming alcohol and 7% to 16%11,17 report smoking tobacco. These may be underestimates because people often underreport alcohol drinking habits.18 Older maternal age, increased education, and longer breastfeeding duration are associated with increased alcohol consumption during lactation.11 Conversely, younger maternal age, lower education, and decreased income are associated with increased tobacco smoking.19
Breastfeeding women report drinking alcohol because of the lack of harmful evidence20 and a mistaken21 belief that alcohol is a galactagogue.20 Alcohol passes quickly through to breastmilk at similar concentrations to maternal blood alcohol concentration22 and reduces milk production.21 Although drinking alcohol immediately after feeding minimizes ethanol exposure,23 not all women use this technique,16,20 and unpredictable infant feeding can mar such attempts.20 Expressing and discarding “contaminated” breastmilk does not reduce ethanol concentration because this is related to maternal blood alcohol concentration.7
Nicotine also passes quickly through to breastmilk, in which concentrations may be higher than maternal serum concentrations.8 Nicotine is associated with reduced milk production and changes in breastmilk composition and taste.24 Breastfeeding women report that despite a belief that maternal tobacco smoking is harmful to infants, difficulties curbing addictive behaviors interfere with its cessation.25
Studies in which alcohol consumption or tobacco smoking during lactation are assessed are limited, and conductors of rat studies generally expose dams to larger alcohol quantities than consumed by human mothers. Despite this, available research suggests that alcohol exposure through breastmilk may have negative cognitive consequences for offspring. Research of dams intoxicated while pregnant and lactating found reduced learning in pups.26 This may be because of prenatal exposure alone, however, because Gray et al27 found no decline when alcohol was only given during lactation. Likewise, dam offspring who consumed alcohol while pregnant and nursing had reduced hippocampal neurons,28 cerebellar neurons,29 and increased cerebral cortex cell apoptosis and necrosis.30 Similar neuronal loss and decreased myelination in pup cerebellums was also found after only lactational exposure.31
Human research has largely been focused on disrupted infant sleeping and feeding patterns.5,32 The authors of a case study from 1978,33 however, described an infant who developed a pseudo-Cushing syndrome after high maternal alcohol consumption during lactation but not pregnancy. Symptoms abated after alcohol cessation. Whereas Little et al34,35 found reduced psychomotor scores at 1 year in infants whose mothers drank alcohol while breastfeeding, the authors of more recent studies found no reduction in developmental scores.16
Rat studies of nicotine intake during lactation have revealed reversible hypothyroidism in offspring.36,37 Although hypothyroidism is associated with cognitive deficits in humans,38 Gaworski et al39 found that pups exposed to nicotine during pregnancy and lactation had intact learning and memory.39 In a study of dams exposed to nicotine during pregnancy and lactation, offspring had delayed muscarinic receptor development.40 Because decreased acetylcholine transmission is associated with cognitive decline,41 this could potentially impact cognitive development.
Human studies in which relationships between tobacco smoking during lactation and childhood developmental outcomes are assessed are scant.42,43 Women who smoke may have infants of lower birth weight44 and wean infants earlier.45 Because low birth weight46 and shorter breastfeeding duration47 are associated with decreased cognition, these factors alone could reduce infant cognition. Laurberg et al48 found that smoking tobacco while lactating caused dose-dependent reductions in milk iodine content and increased children’s risk of iodine deficiency. Although iodine deficiency could theoretically result in cognitive impairment,49 this was not examined.48
In the context of available research, our aim in the current study was to assess whether drinking alcohol or smoking tobacco during lactation adversely impacts cognitive outcomes in children. It was hypothesized that alcohol and nicotine use would result in lower cognitive scores in a dose-dependent manner independent of pregnancy use.
Methods
Ethics
Ethics approval was obtained from Macquarie University Human Research Ethics Committee.
Study Design and Data Source
Data were sourced from Growing Up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). Detailed information regarding LSAC can be found on the LSAC Web site.50 Briefly, LSAC is a longitudinal study of Australian children and their families, conducted by the Australian Government Department of Social Services, the Australian Institute of Family Studies, and the Australian Bureau of Statistics. The researchers aim to examine impacts of social and cultural variables on a range of physical and psychological health and developmental outcomes.51
Study Cohort
Two cohorts (B and K) were recruited into LSAC at wave 1 in 2004. Cohort K was not included in current analyses because the children were 4 to 5 years old at recruitment. Cohort B comprised 5107 infants and caregivers who were managed over time every 2 years. Demographic, lifestyle, cognitive, academic, and developmental variables were collected at each of the 6 waves available for data analyses52,53; further recruitment details are available in LSAC Technical Paper No. 1.54
Breastfeeding
Caregivers were asked whether infants were being breastfed at wave 1 and whether they had ever been breastfed.55 This allowed stratification into wave 1 breastfeeding infants and infants who had been breastfed at any time.
Predictor Variables
Mothers were asked a modified version of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test Alcohol Consumption Questions (AUDIT-C)56,57 at wave 1 (Table 1). Respondents who answered “never” to question 1 were assigned a score of 0 for question 2. Scores were summed to create a total score (range: 1–19). Higher scores indicated increased or riskier alcohol consumption. Pregnancy alcohol was recorded retrospectively (Table 1). The average number of days pregnant mothers drank alcohol per week was calculated by averaging trimester results. Mothers were asked how many cigarettes they smoked on average per day at wave 1 and how many cigarettes they smoked on average per day during pregnancy.55
Frequencies for Each of the Responses and Missing Data for Categorical Predictor, Control, and Outcome Variables Before Data Imputation (N = 5107)
Variable . | N (%) . |
---|---|
Sex of child | |
Male | 2608 (51.1) |
Female | 2499 (48.9) |
Missing | 0 (0.0) |
Wave 1 child still breastfeeding | |
Yes | 2007 (39.3) |
No | 3096 (60.6) |
Missing | 4 (0.1) |
Child was breastfed at any time | |
Yes | 4685 (91.7) |
No | 418 (8.2) |
Missing | 4 (0.1) |
Mother’s trimester 1 d per wk drinking alcohol | |
0 or occasional | 3934 (77) |
1 | 141 (2.8) |
2 | 85 (1.7) |
3 | 32 (0.6) |
4 | 17 (0.3) |
5 | 12 (0.2) |
6 | 5 (0.1) |
7 | 4 (0.1) |
Missing | 877 (17.2) |
Mother’s trimester 2 d per wk drinking alcohol | |
0 or occasional | 3823 (74.9) |
1 | 206 (4.0) |
2 | 118 (2.3) |
3 | 48 (0.9) |
4 | 19 (0.4) |
5 | 9 (0.2) |
6 | 5 (0.1) |
7 | 3 (0.1) |
Missing | 876 (17.2) |
Mother’s trimester 3 d per wk drinking alcohol | |
0 or occasional | 3802 (74.4) |
1 | 190 (3.7) |
2 | 133 (2.6) |
3 | 54 (1.1) |
4 | 24 (0.5) |
5 | 14 (0.3) |
6 | 6 (0.1) |
7 | 5 (0.1) |
Missing | 879 (17.2) |
Pregnancy: average No. drinks on drinking d | |
0 or none | 2597 (50.9) |
1 or 2 | 1560 (30.5) |
3 or 4 | 56 (1.1) |
5 or 6 | 7 (0.1) |
7–10 | 3 (0.1) |
11 or more | 5 (0.1) |
Missing | 879 (17.2) |
Mother’s level of education | |
Never attended or still attending school | 4 (0.1) |
≤Year 8 | 83 (1.6) |
Year 9 or Eq | 161 (3.2) |
Year 10 or Eq | 820 (16.1) |
Year 11 or Eq | 539 (10.6) |
Year 12 or Eq | 1813 (35.5) |
Bachelor degree | 998 (19.5) |
Graduate diploma or certificate | 319 (6.2) |
Postgraduate degree | 361 (7.1) |
Missing | 9 (0.2) |
Combined family income (Australian dollars) | |
≥$2400 per wk or ≥$124 800 per y | 384 (7.5) |
$2200–$2399 per wk or $114 400–$124 799 per y | 106 (2.1) |
$2000–$2199 per wk or $104 000–$114 399 per y | 158 (3.1) |
$1500–$1999 per wk or $78 000–$103 999 per y | 674 (13.2) |
$1000–$1499 per wk or $52 000–$77 999 per y | 1295 (25.4) |
$800–$999 per wk or $41 600–$51 999 per y | 685 (13.4) |
$700–$799 per wk or $36 400–$41 599 per y | 367 (7.2) |
$600–$699 per wk or $31 200–$36 399 per y | 287 (5.6) |
$500–$599 per wk or $26 000–$31 199 per y | 266 (5.2) |
$400–$499 per wk or $20 800–$25 999 per y | 273 (5.3) |
$300–$399 per wk or $15 600–$20 799 per y | 231 (4.5) |
$200–$299 per wk or $10 400–$15 599 per y | 78 (1.5) |
$100–$199 per wk or $5200–$10 399 per y | 17 (0.3) |
$50–$99 per wk or $2600–$5199 per y | 7 (0.1) |
$1–$49 per wk or $1–$2599 per y | 1 (<0.1) |
Nil income | 2 (<0.1) |
Negative income | 4 (0.1) |
Missing | 272 (5.3) |
Mother’s frequency of drinking alcohol (modified AUDIT-C question 1) | |
Never | 387 (7.6) |
Not in the last y | 414 (8.1) |
Monthly or less | 1234 (24.2) |
2–3 times a mo | 582 (11.4) |
Once a wk | 590 (11.6) |
2–3 times a wk | 604 (11.8) |
4–6 times a wk | 303 (5.9) |
Every d | 91 (1.8) |
Missing | 902 (17.7) |
Mother’s average No. drinks when drinking (modified AUDIT-C question 2) | |
0 | 387 (7.6) |
1 or 2 | 2484 (48.6) |
3 or 4 | 611 (12.0) |
5 or 6 | 184 (3.6) |
7–10 | 64 (1.3) |
≥11 | 17 (0.3) |
Missing | 1360 (26.6) |
Mother’s frequency of drinking ≥5 drinks in 1 sitting (modified AUDIT-C question 3) | |
Not in the last y | 2879 (56.4) |
Monthly or less | 1022 (20.0) |
2 or 3 times a mo | 148 (2.9) |
Once a wk | 95 (1.9) |
2–3 times a wk | 46 (0.9) |
4–6 times a wk | 7 (0.1) |
Every d | 1 (0.1) |
Missing | 909 (17.8) |
Head injury wave 3a | |
Yes | 7 (0.1) |
No | 4379 (85.7) |
Missing | 721 (14.1) |
Head injury wave 4a | |
Yes | 19 (0.4) |
No | 4223 (82.7) |
Missing | 865 (16.9) |
Head injury wave 5a | |
Yes | 24 (0.5) |
No | 4024 (78.8) |
Missing | 1059 (20.7) |
Head injury wave 6a | |
Yes | 28 (0.5) |
No | 3670 (71.9) |
Missing | 1409 (27.6) |
Learning difficulty wave 3 | |
Yes | 84 (1.6) |
No | 4302 (84.2) |
Missing | 721 (14.1) |
Learning difficulty wave 4 | |
Yes | 25 (0.5) |
No | 4217 (82.6) |
Missing | 865 (16.9) |
Learning difficulty wave 5 | |
Yes | 92 (1.8) |
No | 3955 (77.4) |
Missing | 1060 (20.8) |
Learning difficulty wave 6 | |
Yes | 107 (2.1) |
No | 3670 (71.9) |
Missing | 1409 (27.6) |
Variable . | N (%) . |
---|---|
Sex of child | |
Male | 2608 (51.1) |
Female | 2499 (48.9) |
Missing | 0 (0.0) |
Wave 1 child still breastfeeding | |
Yes | 2007 (39.3) |
No | 3096 (60.6) |
Missing | 4 (0.1) |
Child was breastfed at any time | |
Yes | 4685 (91.7) |
No | 418 (8.2) |
Missing | 4 (0.1) |
Mother’s trimester 1 d per wk drinking alcohol | |
0 or occasional | 3934 (77) |
1 | 141 (2.8) |
2 | 85 (1.7) |
3 | 32 (0.6) |
4 | 17 (0.3) |
5 | 12 (0.2) |
6 | 5 (0.1) |
7 | 4 (0.1) |
Missing | 877 (17.2) |
Mother’s trimester 2 d per wk drinking alcohol | |
0 or occasional | 3823 (74.9) |
1 | 206 (4.0) |
2 | 118 (2.3) |
3 | 48 (0.9) |
4 | 19 (0.4) |
5 | 9 (0.2) |
6 | 5 (0.1) |
7 | 3 (0.1) |
Missing | 876 (17.2) |
Mother’s trimester 3 d per wk drinking alcohol | |
0 or occasional | 3802 (74.4) |
1 | 190 (3.7) |
2 | 133 (2.6) |
3 | 54 (1.1) |
4 | 24 (0.5) |
5 | 14 (0.3) |
6 | 6 (0.1) |
7 | 5 (0.1) |
Missing | 879 (17.2) |
Pregnancy: average No. drinks on drinking d | |
0 or none | 2597 (50.9) |
1 or 2 | 1560 (30.5) |
3 or 4 | 56 (1.1) |
5 or 6 | 7 (0.1) |
7–10 | 3 (0.1) |
11 or more | 5 (0.1) |
Missing | 879 (17.2) |
Mother’s level of education | |
Never attended or still attending school | 4 (0.1) |
≤Year 8 | 83 (1.6) |
Year 9 or Eq | 161 (3.2) |
Year 10 or Eq | 820 (16.1) |
Year 11 or Eq | 539 (10.6) |
Year 12 or Eq | 1813 (35.5) |
Bachelor degree | 998 (19.5) |
Graduate diploma or certificate | 319 (6.2) |
Postgraduate degree | 361 (7.1) |
Missing | 9 (0.2) |
Combined family income (Australian dollars) | |
≥$2400 per wk or ≥$124 800 per y | 384 (7.5) |
$2200–$2399 per wk or $114 400–$124 799 per y | 106 (2.1) |
$2000–$2199 per wk or $104 000–$114 399 per y | 158 (3.1) |
$1500–$1999 per wk or $78 000–$103 999 per y | 674 (13.2) |
$1000–$1499 per wk or $52 000–$77 999 per y | 1295 (25.4) |
$800–$999 per wk or $41 600–$51 999 per y | 685 (13.4) |
$700–$799 per wk or $36 400–$41 599 per y | 367 (7.2) |
$600–$699 per wk or $31 200–$36 399 per y | 287 (5.6) |
$500–$599 per wk or $26 000–$31 199 per y | 266 (5.2) |
$400–$499 per wk or $20 800–$25 999 per y | 273 (5.3) |
$300–$399 per wk or $15 600–$20 799 per y | 231 (4.5) |
$200–$299 per wk or $10 400–$15 599 per y | 78 (1.5) |
$100–$199 per wk or $5200–$10 399 per y | 17 (0.3) |
$50–$99 per wk or $2600–$5199 per y | 7 (0.1) |
$1–$49 per wk or $1–$2599 per y | 1 (<0.1) |
Nil income | 2 (<0.1) |
Negative income | 4 (0.1) |
Missing | 272 (5.3) |
Mother’s frequency of drinking alcohol (modified AUDIT-C question 1) | |
Never | 387 (7.6) |
Not in the last y | 414 (8.1) |
Monthly or less | 1234 (24.2) |
2–3 times a mo | 582 (11.4) |
Once a wk | 590 (11.6) |
2–3 times a wk | 604 (11.8) |
4–6 times a wk | 303 (5.9) |
Every d | 91 (1.8) |
Missing | 902 (17.7) |
Mother’s average No. drinks when drinking (modified AUDIT-C question 2) | |
0 | 387 (7.6) |
1 or 2 | 2484 (48.6) |
3 or 4 | 611 (12.0) |
5 or 6 | 184 (3.6) |
7–10 | 64 (1.3) |
≥11 | 17 (0.3) |
Missing | 1360 (26.6) |
Mother’s frequency of drinking ≥5 drinks in 1 sitting (modified AUDIT-C question 3) | |
Not in the last y | 2879 (56.4) |
Monthly or less | 1022 (20.0) |
2 or 3 times a mo | 148 (2.9) |
Once a wk | 95 (1.9) |
2–3 times a wk | 46 (0.9) |
4–6 times a wk | 7 (0.1) |
Every d | 1 (0.1) |
Missing | 909 (17.8) |
Head injury wave 3a | |
Yes | 7 (0.1) |
No | 4379 (85.7) |
Missing | 721 (14.1) |
Head injury wave 4a | |
Yes | 19 (0.4) |
No | 4223 (82.7) |
Missing | 865 (16.9) |
Head injury wave 5a | |
Yes | 24 (0.5) |
No | 4024 (78.8) |
Missing | 1059 (20.7) |
Head injury wave 6a | |
Yes | 28 (0.5) |
No | 3670 (71.9) |
Missing | 1409 (27.6) |
Learning difficulty wave 3 | |
Yes | 84 (1.6) |
No | 4302 (84.2) |
Missing | 721 (14.1) |
Learning difficulty wave 4 | |
Yes | 25 (0.5) |
No | 4217 (82.6) |
Missing | 865 (16.9) |
Learning difficulty wave 5 | |
Yes | 92 (1.8) |
No | 3955 (77.4) |
Missing | 1060 (20.8) |
Learning difficulty wave 6 | |
Yes | 107 (2.1) |
No | 3670 (71.9) |
Missing | 1409 (27.6) |
Head injuries noted at earlier waves were automatically recorded at all later waves.
Outcome Variables
Outcome variables were as follows:
Transformed scores from an adapted Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition (PPVT-III)58,59 (waves 3, 4 and 5). Higher scores indicated increased vocabulary.
Raw scores from the Matrix Reasoning (MR) subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fourth Edition60 (waves 4, 5 and 6). Higher scores indicated increased nonverbal reasoning.
Raw scores from the Who Am I? test (WAI),59,61,62 with 1 change to item 1163 (wave 3). Higher scores indicated increases in cognitive processes underlying early literacy and numeracy.
Control Variables
Infant sex was controlled for because of postulated sex differences in cognitive abilities.64 Infant and maternal age were included because maternal age is associated with alcohol and cigarette use during lactation,11,19 and cognition varies with age.65 Because women who smoke tend to have lower birth weight infants44 and lower birth weight is associated with poorer cognition,46 birth weight was also included as a control variable. Breastfeeding duration was controlled for because earlier weaning is associated with lowered cognition47 as well as maternal tobacco smoking.45
Combined household income and maternal education were included because both are associated with tobacco smoking and alcohol consumption during lactation11,19 as well as cognitive outcomes.66 Learning difficulties (delay relative to peers in the context of a medical condition) and brain injuries (concussion and/or internal head injury requiring medical attention) were included as control variables because both can alter cognitive profiles.67,68 The primary language spoken at home was only included for the adapted PPVT-III and WAI analyses because both are language reliant.
Statistical Analyses
Data were analyzed by using IBM SPSS version 24 (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM Corporation). Missing data from all included variables were imputed by using multiple imputation (MI). To reduce estimate bias, arithmetically derived variables were calculated after MI of individual items.69 Thirty imputations were used because the highest proportion of missing data for any variable was 30% (Supplemental Information). When missing data are ˂50%, matching the imputation number to missing data percentage increases efficiency and replicability of data.70 Imputations were constrained to variable ranges where applicable and were not rounded to integers to reduce estimate bias.71 Skewed independent variables were not transformed before MI because this can produce poorer estimates.72
Multivariable linear regression analyses were performed including each of the predictor and control variables. Multicollinearity was assessed by using the variance inflation factor (VIF). A VIF ˂10 was considered acceptable.73 Analyses were conducted separately for each outcome variable at each wave. Only data from biological mothers were included. Infants still being breastfed at wave 1 and infants who had been breastfed at any time were analyzed separately. The Benjamini and Hochberg74 procedure was used to correct for type I error (α = .05). This procedure is superior to the Bonferroni correction in preserving power.74
Results
Descriptive Statistics
Means, SDs, Ranges, and Missing Data for Quantitative Predictor, Control, and Outcome Variables Before Data Imputation (N = 5107)
Variable . | Mean (SD) . | Range . | Missing Data N (%) . |
---|---|---|---|
Child’s birth wt, g | 3410.15 (568.83) | 382.00–5440.00 | 35 (0.7) |
Child’s age wave 1, y | 0.77 (0.21) | 0.33–1.59 | 0 (0) |
Child’s age wave 3, y | 4.88 (0.25) | 4.16–5.84 | 721 (14) |
Child’s age wave 4, y | 6.91 (0.29) | 6.16–7.92 | 865 (17) |
Child’s age wave 5, y | 8.99 (0.32) | 8.08–9.84 | 1022 (20) |
Child’s age wave 6, y | 11.01 (0.33) | 10.08–11.84 | 1343 (26) |
Mother’s age wave 1, y | 31.48 (5.47) | 15.66–63.92 | 7 (0.1) |
Average daily cigarettes while pregnant | 1.19 (3.81) | 0.00–55.00 | 1041 (20) |
Mother’s average daily cigarettes wave 1 | 2.09 (5.25) | 0.00–40.0 | 823 (16) |
WAI score wave 3 | 26.94 (7.10) | 1.00–43.00 | 910 (18) |
Adapted PPVT-III score wave 3 | 65.16 (5.99) | 34.00–85.00 | 841 (17) |
Adapted PPVT-III score wave 4 | 74.43 (5.15) | 36.00–92.00 | 922 (18) |
Adapted PPVT-III score wave 5 | 79.10 (4.83) | 52.00–106.00 | 1093 (21) |
MR score wave 4 | 14.00 (4.67) | 3.00–30.00 | 927 (18) |
MR score wave 5 | 19.55 (4.83) | 1.00–32.00 | 1107 (22) |
MR score wave 6 | 22.08 (5.07) | 1.00–33.00 | 1509 (30) |
Breastfeeding duration, d | 228.89 (202.88) | 0.00–1157 | 583 (11) |
Variable . | Mean (SD) . | Range . | Missing Data N (%) . |
---|---|---|---|
Child’s birth wt, g | 3410.15 (568.83) | 382.00–5440.00 | 35 (0.7) |
Child’s age wave 1, y | 0.77 (0.21) | 0.33–1.59 | 0 (0) |
Child’s age wave 3, y | 4.88 (0.25) | 4.16–5.84 | 721 (14) |
Child’s age wave 4, y | 6.91 (0.29) | 6.16–7.92 | 865 (17) |
Child’s age wave 5, y | 8.99 (0.32) | 8.08–9.84 | 1022 (20) |
Child’s age wave 6, y | 11.01 (0.33) | 10.08–11.84 | 1343 (26) |
Mother’s age wave 1, y | 31.48 (5.47) | 15.66–63.92 | 7 (0.1) |
Average daily cigarettes while pregnant | 1.19 (3.81) | 0.00–55.00 | 1041 (20) |
Mother’s average daily cigarettes wave 1 | 2.09 (5.25) | 0.00–40.0 | 823 (16) |
WAI score wave 3 | 26.94 (7.10) | 1.00–43.00 | 910 (18) |
Adapted PPVT-III score wave 3 | 65.16 (5.99) | 34.00–85.00 | 841 (17) |
Adapted PPVT-III score wave 4 | 74.43 (5.15) | 36.00–92.00 | 922 (18) |
Adapted PPVT-III score wave 5 | 79.10 (4.83) | 52.00–106.00 | 1093 (21) |
MR score wave 4 | 14.00 (4.67) | 3.00–30.00 | 927 (18) |
MR score wave 5 | 19.55 (4.83) | 1.00–32.00 | 1107 (22) |
MR score wave 6 | 22.08 (5.07) | 1.00–33.00 | 1509 (30) |
Breastfeeding duration, d | 228.89 (202.88) | 0.00–1157 | 583 (11) |
Power Analyses
Sixteen female caregivers who were not biological mothers were excluded after MI. This left 2009 breastfed infants and 4679 infants who had been breastfed at some time. Power analyses revealed that with 2009 subjects, 99.57% (14 independent variables) and 99.49% (15 independent variables) power was achieved to detect a small effect size (Cohen's d = 0.2, α = .05). A sample size of 4679 provided ˃99% power to detect an effect size of Cohen's d = 0.2 (α = .05), with 14 or 15 independent variables.75,76
Wave 1 Maternal Alcohol Consumption (Before MI)
Modified AUDIT-C scores of wave 1 breastfeeding mothers (mean = 5.55; SD = 2.46; 95% confidence interval [CI]: 5.42 to 5.68) were lower than scores for mothers who were not breastfeeding (mean = 6.13; SD = 2.72; 95% CI: 6.02 to 6.24; P ˂ .001; Cohen's d = 0.22). There was no statistically significant difference in modified AUDIT-C scores between mothers who had breastfed (n = 3443; mean = 5.90; SD = 2.60; 95% CI: 5.81 to 5.99) and had never breastfed their infants (mean = 5.81; SD = 3.04; 95% CI: 5.50 to 6.13; P = .60; Cohen's d = 0.03).
Wave 1 Maternal Tobacco Smoking (Before MI)
Breastfeeding wave 1 mothers smoked fewer cigarettes on average per day (mean = 1.06; SD = 3.67; 95% CI: 0.77 to 1.25) than women who were not breastfeeding (mean = 2.84; SD = 5.97; 95% CI: 2.64 to 3.04; P ˂ .001; Cohen's d = 0.37). Mothers whose infants had been breastfed at some time, smoked fewer cigarettes on average per day (mean = 1.85; SD = 4.87; 95% CI: 1.69 to 2.01) than mothers whose infants had never breastfed (mean = 5.00; SD = 8.02; 95% CI: 4.43 to 5.57; P ˂ .001; Cohen's d = 0.47).
Missing Data
Little’s missing completely at random test revealed that data were not missing completely at random, P = ˂ .001. Previous LSAC analysis revealed that more poorly educated caregivers tended to drop out of the study,77 suggesting that missing data were related to independent variables and suitable for MI.78
Linear Regression: MR Scores
Full wave 5 to 6 results are available in Supplemental Table 5.
Infants Breastfeeding at Wave 1
Models explained 1% to 19%, 1% to 26%, and 2% to 14% of variance, respectively, across waves for each imputation. Older wave 4 child age was associated with increased wave 4 MR scores. Increased and/or riskier maternal alcohol consumption while breastfeeding was associated with decreased wave 4 MR scores. This was no longer statistically significant, however, after multiple comparison adjustment. No other statistically significant relationships were observed. Full results are shown in Table 3.
Infants Being Breastfed at Wave 1: Regression Coefficients, SEs, CIs, P Values, and Adjusted P Values for Each Predictor and Control Variable for Wave 4 MR Scores (n = 2009)
Variablea . | B Coefficient . | SE . | 95% CI . | P . | Adjusted Pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 3.98 | 3.02 | −1.96 to 9.92 | .19 | N/A |
Child’s age wave 4 | 1.64 | 0.40 | 0.85 to 2.44 | <.001 | <.001 |
Mother’s modified AUDIT-C score wave 1c | −0.12 | 0.06 | −0.23 to −0.01 | .03 | .22 |
Child’s birth wt | <0.001 | <0.001 | −0.001 to −<0.001 | .12 | .48 |
Pregnancy: average d per wk drinking alcohol | 0.28 | 0.19 | −0.09 to 0.65 | .14 | .48 |
Pregnancy: average No. drinks | 0.27 | 0.25 | −0.23 to 0.76 | .30 | .72 |
Learning difficulty wave 4 | −1.72 | 1.67 | −5.10 to 1.66 | .31 | .72 |
Combined family income | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.14 to 0.05 | .36 | .73 |
Mother’s age wave 1 | 0.02 | 0.02 | −0.03 to 0.06 | .44 | .77 |
Average daily cigarettes while pregnant | −0.05 | 0.08 | −0.20 to 0.10 | .53 | .81 |
Head injury wave 4 | 0.93 | 1.65 | −2.40 to 4.25 | .58 | .81 |
Child’s sex | −0.08 | 0.22 | −0.52 to 0.36 | .72 | .81 |
Breastfeeding duration | <0.001 | <0.001 | −<0.001 to <0.001 | .72 | .81 |
Mother’s level of education | 0.02 | 0.08 | −0.14 to 0.19 | .77 | .81 |
Mother’s average daily cigarettes wave 1d | 0.01 | 0.06 | −0.10 to 0.13 | .81 | .81 |
Variablea . | B Coefficient . | SE . | 95% CI . | P . | Adjusted Pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 3.98 | 3.02 | −1.96 to 9.92 | .19 | N/A |
Child’s age wave 4 | 1.64 | 0.40 | 0.85 to 2.44 | <.001 | <.001 |
Mother’s modified AUDIT-C score wave 1c | −0.12 | 0.06 | −0.23 to −0.01 | .03 | .22 |
Child’s birth wt | <0.001 | <0.001 | −0.001 to −<0.001 | .12 | .48 |
Pregnancy: average d per wk drinking alcohol | 0.28 | 0.19 | −0.09 to 0.65 | .14 | .48 |
Pregnancy: average No. drinks | 0.27 | 0.25 | −0.23 to 0.76 | .30 | .72 |
Learning difficulty wave 4 | −1.72 | 1.67 | −5.10 to 1.66 | .31 | .72 |
Combined family income | −0.04 | 0.05 | −0.14 to 0.05 | .36 | .73 |
Mother’s age wave 1 | 0.02 | 0.02 | −0.03 to 0.06 | .44 | .77 |
Average daily cigarettes while pregnant | −0.05 | 0.08 | −0.20 to 0.10 | .53 | .81 |
Head injury wave 4 | 0.93 | 1.65 | −2.40 to 4.25 | .58 | .81 |
Child’s sex | −0.08 | 0.22 | −0.52 to 0.36 | .72 | .81 |
Breastfeeding duration | <0.001 | <0.001 | −<0.001 to <0.001 | .72 | .81 |
Mother’s level of education | 0.02 | 0.08 | −0.14 to 0.19 | .77 | .81 |
Mother’s average daily cigarettes wave 1d | 0.01 | 0.06 | −0.10 to 0.13 | .81 | .81 |
N/A, not applicable.
VIF <10 for all variables.
Benjamini-Hochberg method.
Unadjusted B = −0.045; SE = 0.05; 95% CI: −0.14 to 0.05; P = .35.
Unadjusted B = −0.02; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: −0.08 to 0.04; P = .49.
Infants Who Had Been Breastfed at Any Time
Models explained 2% to 16%, 3% to 25%, and 2% to 14% of variance, respectively, across waves for each imputation. Increased MR wave 4 scores were predicted by increased wave 4 child age. Increased or riskier maternal alcohol consumption was associated with decreased wave 4 MR scores. This relationship remained statistically significant after multiple comparison adjustments. At wave 5, only older wave 5 child age was associated with increased wave 5 MR scores. At wave 6, learning difficulties predicted lower wave 6 MR scores. Full results are shown in Table 4.
Infants Who Had Been Breastfed at Any Time: Regression Coefficients, SEs, CIs, P Values, and Adjusted P Values for Each Predictor and Control Variable for Wave 4 MR Scores (n = 4679)
Variablea . | B Coefficient . | SE . | 95% CI . | P . | Adjusted Pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 2.88 | 2.14 | −1.32 to 7.09 | .18 | N/A |
Child’s age wave 4 | 1.83 | 0.29 | 1.27 to 2.40 | <.001 | <.001 |
Mother’s modified AUDIT-C score wave 1c | −0.11 | 0.03 | −0.18 to −0.04 | .001 | .01 |
Average daily cigarettes while pregnant | −0.05 | 0.03 | −0.12 to 0.02 | .13 | .46 |
Pregnancy: average d per wk drinking alcohol | 0.19 | 0.14 | −0.08 to 0.46 | .16 | .46 |
Child’s birth wt | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 to <0.001 | .17 | .46 |
Pregnancy: average No. drinks | 0.22 | 0.17 | −0.12 to 0.55 | .21 | .48 |
Learning difficulty wave 4 | −1.51 | 1.55 | −4.66 to 1.64 | .34 | .54 |
Mother’s age wave 1 | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.04 to 0.02 | .35 | .54 |
Combined family income | −0.03 | 0.03 | −0.09 to 0.04 | .38 | .54 |
Mother’s average daily cigarettes wave 1d | 0.02 | 0.03 | −0.03 to 0.07 | .39 | .54 |
Breastfeeding duration | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 to 0.001 | .48 | .61 |
Mother’s level of education | 0.04 | 0.06 | −0.08 to 0.15 | .54 | .63 |
Head injury wave 4 | 0.86 | 1.56 | −2.31 to 4.02 | .59 | .63 |
Child’s sex | −0.05 | 0.15 | −0.33 to 0.24 | .76 | .76 |
Variablea . | B Coefficient . | SE . | 95% CI . | P . | Adjusted Pb . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 2.88 | 2.14 | −1.32 to 7.09 | .18 | N/A |
Child’s age wave 4 | 1.83 | 0.29 | 1.27 to 2.40 | <.001 | <.001 |
Mother’s modified AUDIT-C score wave 1c | −0.11 | 0.03 | −0.18 to −0.04 | .001 | .01 |
Average daily cigarettes while pregnant | −0.05 | 0.03 | −0.12 to 0.02 | .13 | .46 |
Pregnancy: average d per wk drinking alcohol | 0.19 | 0.14 | −0.08 to 0.46 | .16 | .46 |
Child’s birth wt | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 to <0.001 | .17 | .46 |
Pregnancy: average No. drinks | 0.22 | 0.17 | −0.12 to 0.55 | .21 | .48 |
Learning difficulty wave 4 | −1.51 | 1.55 | −4.66 to 1.64 | .34 | .54 |
Mother’s age wave 1 | −0.01 | 0.02 | −0.04 to 0.02 | .35 | .54 |
Combined family income | −0.03 | 0.03 | −0.09 to 0.04 | .38 | .54 |
Mother’s average daily cigarettes wave 1d | 0.02 | 0.03 | −0.03 to 0.07 | .39 | .54 |
Breastfeeding duration | <0.001 | <0.001 | <0.001 to 0.001 | .48 | .61 |
Mother’s level of education | 0.04 | 0.06 | −0.08 to 0.15 | .54 | .63 |
Head injury wave 4 | 0.86 | 1.56 | −2.31 to 4.02 | .59 | .63 |
Child’s sex | −0.05 | 0.15 | −0.33 to 0.24 | .76 | .76 |
N/A, not applicable.
VIF <10 for all variables.
Benjamini-Hochberg method.
Unadjusted B = −0.07; SE = 0.03; 95% CI: −0.12 to 0.01; P = .03.
Unadjusted B = −0.01; SE = 0.02; 95% CI: −0.04 to 0.02; P = .50.
Infants Who Had Never Been Breastfed
When assessing infants who had never breastfed, only modified AUDIT-C scores were included as a predictor to maximize power. With 1 independent variable, a sample size of 412 provided 80% power to detect a small effect size (Cohen's d = 0.2; α = .05).75,76 The model accounted for ˂0.001% variance, and modified AUDIT-C scores weren’t associated with wave 4 MR scores (B = −0.02; SE = 0.10; 95% CI = −0.20 to 0.17; P = .87).
Linear Regression: WAI Scores
Full results are available in Supplemental Table 6.
Infants Breastfeeding at Wave 1
The model explained 7% to 41% of variance across imputations. Older wave 3 child age was associated with increased WAI scores. Learning difficulties were also associated with decreased WAI scores. There was no associated between maternal alcohol consumption or tobacco smoking and WAI scores.
Infants Who Had Been Breastfed at Any Time
The model explained 9% to 41% of variance across imputations. Older wave 3 child age predicted higher WAI scores. Learning difficulties predicted lower WAI scores. No other variables were statistically significant.
Linear Regression: Adapted PPVT-III Scores
Full results are available in Supplemental Table 7.
Infants Breastfeeding at Wave 1
Models explained 4% to 17%, 2% to 15%, and 1% to 25% of variance, respectively, across waves for each imputation. Learning difficulties were associated with lower wave 3–adapted PPVT-III scores. Older wave 4 child age was associated with higher wave 4 scores. No other variables were statistically significant.
Infants Who Had Been Breastfed at Any Time
Models explained 3% to 13%, 2% to 14%, and 2% to 18% of variance, respectively, across waves for each imputation. Learning difficulties were associated with lower wave 3–adapted PPVT-III scores. Older wave 3 child age was associated with higher wave 4 scores. Other variables were not statistically significant.
Discussion
This is the first study in which associations between alcohol exposure through breastmilk and cognition in children are examined. Greater or riskier maternal alcohol intake was associated with decreased nonverbal reasoning at 6 to 7 years in a dose-dependent manner. This was independent of prenatal alcohol consumption, sex, child and maternal age, income, birth weight, breastfeeding duration, learning delay, head injury, and pregnancy and breastfeeding tobacco smoking. Although this relationship was found in wave 1 breastfeeding children, with multiple comparison adjustment it was no longer statistically significant. In children who had been breastfed at any time, however, this association remained statistically significant after adjustment.
There was no relationship between maternal alcohol consumption and MR scores in infants who had never breastfed. This suggests that alcohol exposure through breastmilk was responsible for cognitive reductions in breastfed infants rather than psychosocial or environmental factors surrounding maternal alcohol consumption. This supports the suggestion that alcohol exposure through breastmilk can reduce cognition in children.
No relationship between maternal tobacco smoking and MR scores was found in wave 1 breastfed infants or infants who had been breastfed at any time at any wave. Likewise, no associations were observed between maternal alcohol or cigarette use and adapted PPVT-III or WAI scores for either group at any wave. The suggestion that smoking tobacco during lactation reduces cognition in children was not supported.
Although no directly comparable previous research exists, Little et al34 found reduced developmental scores in children of mothers who drank while breastfeeding. The authors of a case study of a breastfed infant who developed a pseudo-Cushing syndrome also found that symptoms abated once maternal alcohol was ceased. Because the infant had no prenatal alcohol exposure, this suggests that alcohol exposure through breastmilk can directly impact children’s development.33 Additionally, prenatal alcohol exposure is more consistently associated with executive dysfunction than language or numeracy impairments.4 This is consistent with the observed reductions in MR scores but not the language- or numeracy-based measures.
Although current analyses found an association between increased or riskier alcohol consumption while breastfeeding and MR scores, the mechanism is unclear. Consistent with animal models, ethanol in breastmilk may interfere with normal brain development.28,–31 Increased cerebral cortex apoptosis and necrosis,30 for example, may disrupt higher order executive skills relied on in reasoning tasks. Likewise, decreased myelination31 could reduce the processing speed needed to problem solve quickly. Alternatively, reduced cognition may be secondary to changes in feeding and sleeping.5,32 Alterations in nutritional intake and sleep patterns may modify brain development or cause behavioral changes that reduce exposure to enriching stimuli.
The relationship between increased alcohol exposure through breastmilk and decreased cognition was not evident at waves 5 to 6. Because older age also ceased to be predictive of wave 6 MR scores, the effects of age and alcohol may be mediated by factors such as increased education. Because learning difficulties were associated with lower wave 6 MR scores, alcohol may also indirectly alter cognition by contributing to developmental disorders in older children.
Interestingly, despite known teratogenic effects,1,3 no association between prenatal alcohol exposure and children’s cognition was observed. This may be related to the small quantities and infrequency of prenatal alcohol consumption. Furthermore, prenatal binge drinking of alcohol was not recorded and has been associated with reduced cognition in children.2 The size of the observed relationship between alcohol exposure through breastmilk and cognition was also small, and clinical implications may be limited unless mothers drink large quantities or frequently binge drink. Additionally, given this small effect, the sample size of infants who had never breastfed may have been too small to detect a relationship, despite attempts to maximize power.
There are several other limitations. The frequency and quantity of milk consumed by infants was not recorded, nor was the timing of alcohol consumption or the amount of ethanol in breastmilk. The impact of this is unknown, however, because not all women time their alcohol consumption to limit alcohol exposure, and unpredictable infant feeding patterns can interfere with timing attempts.20
Although wave 1 alcohol consumption was recorded contemporaneously, pregnancy alcohol measures were retrospective. Although both are likely to be underestimates,18 retrospective measures may be even less accurate. Furthermore, measures of wave 1 and pregnancy alcohol differed, preventing direct comparisons.
Cognitive measures available from LSAC were limited. A more comprehensive assessment of cognition including executive functioning, processing speed, learning and memory, visuospatial abilities, and basic as well as complex attention would have been beneficial.
Conclusions
Increased or riskier maternal alcohol consumption during lactation was associated with dose-dependent reductions in abstract reasoning at age 6 to 7 years. This relationship was not observed in infants who had never breastfed, suggesting a direct relationship between alcohol exposure through breastmilk and decreased cognition. The association was not evident at ages 8 to 11 years, which may relate to increased education in older children. Alternatively, because learning difficulties predicted lower MR scores at ages 10 to 11 years, alcohol may be associated with developmental disorders that contribute to these difficulties. Further analyses of LSAC data are planned to assess this possibility as well as relationships between alcohol exposure through breastmilk and academic, physical, and behavioral outcomes in children. Future research should also be focused on direct measures of alcohol in breastmilk and use more comprehensive cognitive assessments.
- AUDIT-C
Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test Alcohol Consumption Questions
- CI
confidence interval
- LSAC
Growing Up in Australia, The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children
- MI
multiple imputation
- MR
Matrix Reasoning
- PPVT-III
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test–Third Edition
- VIF
variance inflation factor
- WAI
Who Am I? test
Mr Gibson conceptualized the research topic, designed the analyses, conducted all analyses, and drafted the initial manuscript; Prof Porter assisted in the design of the analyses; and all authors reviewed and revised the manuscript and approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
FUNDING: No external funding.
COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2018-1377.
Acknowledgments
In this article, we used unit record data from Growing Up in Australia, the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. The study was conducted in partnership between the Department of Social Services, the Australian Institute of Family Studies, and the Australian Bureau of Statistics. The findings and views reported in this article are those of the author and should not be attributed to Department of Social Services, Australian Institute of Family Studies, or the Australian Bureau of Statistics.
References
Competing Interests
POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.
FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.
Comments
RE: Comment from May et al.
The paper by May et al.1 is indeed an important paper with very interesting findings, and we apologise for not discussing them in our current paper.
The Test for Reception of Grammar (TROG) used by May et al, however, is not a measure of IQ or cognition. The TROG is a test of English grammar comprehension. The term IQ has a formal meaning, referring specifically to the Intelligence Quotient obtained by administering the full suite of subtests within the Wechsler batteries (or other versions such as Stanford-Binet). It is also possible to obtain a verbal IQ, if all of the subtests within the verbal domain of the Wechsler batteries are administered. These verbal subtests, however, relate to verbal reasoning abilities, not grammar or other aspects of English language.
English grammar comprehension is not regarded by neuropsychologists as a cognitive domain, since language ability is highly culture specific and dependent on many non-cognitive abilities. While cognition certainly contributes to language, it is incorrect to infer cognitive deficits from impairments in a test of grammar on its own. Even in the current paper, the Matrix Reasoning test in which the effect was observed, is the only direct measure of cognition utilised. The other two measures were both language based and only very loosely defined as tests of cognition.
None of this, however, detracts from the important findings of May et al, and May et al. will indeed be referenced and discussed in an upcoming paper in which grammar comprehension has direct relevance to the topic of interest.
1. May PA, Hasken JM, Blankenship J, et al. Breastfeeding and maternal alcohol use: Prevalence and effects on child outcomes and fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Reproductive toxicology. 2016;63:13-21.
RE: Comment on Gibson and Porter
Several members of our research team read with interest the recent article by Gibson and Porter in Pediatrics (Vol. 142(2), August, 2018)1 entitled “Drinking and Smoking while Breastfeeding and Later Cognition in Children.” The authors suggest that this is the first article to report an association between maternal drinking during the period of breastfeeding and cognitive performance in children ages 6 - 7 years. They found a significant association between “increased or riskier… maternal alcohol consumption” (p. 1) in a child’s first year of life and child cognition as measured by poorer performance on Matrix Reasoning scores at age 6 to 7 years. They concluded that riskier drinking exposes “infants to alcohol through breastmilk [which] may cause dose-dependent reductions in their cognitive abilities” (p.1). We agree with these conclusions.
However, this is not the first documented association between alcohol in breast milk and decreased cognitive ability or other negative outcomes in children. Members of our research team published a manuscript in Reproductive Toxicology in 2016 (Vol. 63, pp, 13-19)2 entitled “Breastfeeding and Maternal Alcohol Use: Prevalence and Effects on Child Outcomes and Fetal Alcohol Spectrum Disorders.” In this paper we reported a significant association in case control comparisons between postpartum drinkers who breastfed their children and lower IQ in their children at ages 6 – 7 years. We also reported a significantly negative effect on physical growth and development as measured by weight, overall minor anomalies and the likelihood of a diagnosis on the continuum of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASD). Controlling for nine confounders of prenatal risk, including four measures of quantity, frequency, and timing of alcohol consumption in the index pregnancies, “mothers who drank postpartum and breastfed their child were 6.4 times more likely to have a child with FASD than breastfeeding mothers who abstained from alcohol while breastfeeding.” (p. 13) Furthermore, after controlling for these confounders “drinking alcohol during the breastfeeding period independently increased the variance in predicting a FASD diagnosis by 2.5%” (p. 19). FASD diagnoses require significant deficiencies in neurobehavior and quite frequently restriction of physical growth and increased dysmorphology.
In our opinion, it is appropriate for the authors to re-address their review of the literature in this manuscript to include in the discussion and conclusions some mention of our work and comparison of the findings from both studies. While we are gratified that their data and results are clearly supportive of our findings, accuracy and completeness dictate that readers be fully informed of all literature on this topic.
References:
1. Gibson L, Porter M. Drinking or Smoking While Breastfeeding and Later Cognition in Children. Pediatrics. 2018;142(2).
2. May PA, Hasken JM, Blankenship J, et al. Breastfeeding and maternal alcohol use: Prevalence and effects on child outcomes and fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. Reprod Toxicol. 2016;63:13-21. doi:10.1016/j.reprotox.2016.05.002
RE: Not all individuals are born equal
First of all, this argument on the relation between drinking or smoking while breastfeeding and later cognition in children, it must be well placed, considering children Inherited Real Risk of brain degenerative disorders (1-5). Briefly, this study is fundamentally biased. Secondly, to affirm the mental condition of a person, one must have the capacity, that is to say, one has to be able to bedside evaluate the cerebral microcirculation, i.e., flow-motion in smallest blood vessels, in the various brain areas, above-cited, inherited real risk depend on (1-3). Finally the Psychokinetic Diagnostic can allow physician to express a similar judgment even if the physician is far miles from the patient to be visited.
References.
1) Sergio Stagnaro. Introduzione alla Microangiologia Clinica 10 dicembre 2011. www.sisbq.org, http://www.sisbq.org/uploads/5/6/8/7/5687930/mc_intro.pdf
2) Sergio Stagnaro - Marina Neri Stagnaro. Microangiologia Clinica. A cura di Simone Caramel. e-book, www.sisbq.org,
http://www.sisbq.org/uploads/5/6/8/7/5687930/microangiologiaclinicasbq20...
3)Stagnaro S., Percussione Ascoltata degli Attacchi Ischemici Transitori. Ruolo dei Potenziali Cerebrali Evocati. Min. Med. 76, 1211, 1985 [Medline]
4) Marco Marchionni, Simone Caramel, Sergio Stagnaro. The Role of ‘Modified Mediterranean Diet’ and Quantum Therapy In Alzheimer’s Disease Primary Prevention. Letter to the Editor, The Journal of Nutrition, Health & Aging, Volume 18, Number 1, 2014, Springer Ed. http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s12603-013-0435-7 [Medline]
5) Stagnaro Sergio. Inherited Real Risk of Brain Disorders. www.plos.org, 24 July 2009. http://www.plosone.org/article/comments/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.p...
Alcohol Use While Breastfeeding and Cognitive Delays
Gibson et al highlight an association between alcohol use while breastfeeding and cognitive delays in infants in the August issue.1 We are concerned, however, that the authors’ conclusions are vastly overstated and may mislead providers and parents regarding potential risks of alcohol use during lactation.
First, infant exposure to alcohol is unknown in this study. The authors conducted an analysis using an existing database in which mothers were asked at the beginning of the study (Wave 1) about their current alcohol use. At that time, only a minority of infants were still being breastfed (39.3%) and, while the majority (91.7%) had been breastfed at some point, 52.3% of the total cohort ceased breastfeeding prior to Wave 1 when infants averaged 9.7 months old. Although only trivial amounts of alcohol are excreted into breast milk, the questionnaires did not examine whether mothers breastfed immediately after consuming alcohol or began drinking alcohol days, weeks, or months after stopping breastfeeding the infant.2,3
Second, the purported association between alcohol use during lactation and cognitive delays is likely a statistical anomaly. Of three outcomes assessing cognition, only abstract reasoning scores reached statistical significance. Further, this association only held in the larger cohort of infants who were ever breastfed (including currently breastfed and previously breastfed infants), not in the subset that were currently being breastfed. If the slightly lower abstract reasoning scores were in fact reflective of a true causal relationship between alcohol intake and cognition, that relationship should be stronger in the infants still breastfeeding at the time of reported maternal alcohol consumption, not weaker. It is unclear why the authors chose to group currently breastfed infants with previously breastfed infants for their analyses, when only current alcohol intake was reported at Wave 1, but the fact that their only statistically significant finding (after correction for multiple comparisons) was in the larger cohort suggests that no clear biological mechanism underpinned this finding.
Moreover, the reported association of maternal alcohol intake with cognition in infants who were ever breastfed is of no clinical significance, as this effect was found at approximately 7 years of age but not at 10 years of age. In fact, it’s reassuring that there is no clinically important effect on cognition, as this study had greater than 99% power to detect a small effect size and found no effect in 6 of the 7 outcome-time point combinations they evaluated.
While we applaud the authors for attempting to contribute to the evidence-base around breastfeeding, a more responsible conclusion from the study results is that no significant, consistent effect was seen in measures of cognition amongst breastfed infants whose mothers consumed alcohol. While alcohol dependence and abuse may pose risks to children independent of breastfeeding, this study does not support the claim that alcohol use while lactating leads to worse cognitive outcomes in children. Propagating that claim may cause unnecessary anxiety, guilt, and confusion among parents and providers.
Gibson L, Porter M. Drinking or Smoking While Breastfeeding and Later Cognition in Children. Pediatrics. 2018; 142(2).
Haastrup MB, Pottegård A, Damkier P. Alcohol and breastfeeding. Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol. 2014;114(2):168-73
“Study questionnaires.” The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. https://growingupinaustralia.gov.au/data-and-documentation/study-questio... Accessed August 1, 2018.