Video Abstract

Video Abstract

Close modal
BACKGROUND:

The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends literacy promotion and developmental assessment during well-child visits. Emergent literacy skills are well defined, and the use of early screening has the potential to identify children at risk for reading difficulties and guide intervention before kindergarten.

METHODS:

The Reading House (TRH) is a children’s book designed to screen emergent literacy skills. These are assessed by sharing the book with the child and using a 9-item, scripted scoring form. Get Ready to Read! (GRTR) is a validated measure shown to predict reading outcomes. TRH and GRTR were administered in random order to 278 children (mean: 43.1 ± 5.6 months; 125 boys, 153 girls) during well-child visits at 7 primary care sites. Parent, child, and provider impressions of TRH were also assessed. Analyses included Rasch methods, Spearman-ρ correlations, and logistic regression, including covariates age, sex, and clinic type.

RESULTS:

Psychometric properties were strong, including item difficulty and reliability. Internal consistency was good for new measures (rCo-α = 0.68). The mean TRH score was 4.2 (±2.9; range: 0–14), and mean GRTR was 11.1 (±4.4; range: 1–25). TRH scores were positively correlated with GRTR scores (rs = 0.66; high), female sex, private practice, and child age (P < .001). The relationship remained significant controlling for these covariates (P < .05). The mean TRH administration time was 5:25 minutes (±0:55; range: 3:34–8:32). Parent, child, and provider impressions of TRH were favorable.

CONCLUSIONS:

TRH is a feasible, valid, and enjoyable means by which emergent literacy skills in 3- and 4-year-old children can be directly assessed during primary care.

What’s Known on This Subject:

The American Academy of Pediatrics recommends literacy promotion and developmental assessment at all well-child visits. Emergent literacy skills are well defined, and early screening can identify children at risk, although existing measures rely on parent report or are infeasible for primary care.

What This Study Adds:

The Reading House is a children’s book specially designed to directly screen emergent literacy skills in 3- and 4-year-old children that is feasible for primary care, enjoyable, and useful for families, reveals promising psychometric properties, and complements existing reading programs.

The American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) recommends literacy promotion in primary care beginning as soon as possible after birth1 and stresses the central role of the pediatrician in school readiness.2 Authors of a recent report from the National Center on Learning Disabilities cited improved early screening and expertise of health care providers to recognize early signs of reading difficulties as priorities.3 Pediatric practices are uniquely poised to conduct literacy screening and guidance4,5 given their trusted role and privileged access to families.6,8 However, although reading guidance variably occurs,9 literacy screening is currently not well integrated into primary care,10 potentially resulting in missed opportunities to identify children at risk for difficulties during maximal brain development.11,13 

Emergent literacy is “a developmental continuum (involving) skills, knowledge, and attitudes that are precursors to reading and writing.”14 These accrue during specific age ranges, beginning with nonverbal communication in infancy14,16 and culminating in reading fluency.17 Many children arrive at kindergarten at a disadvantage in reading readiness (25% overall and 50% in poverty scoring “inadequate” in a recent survey18,19) and are increasingly unlikely to catch up with peers as academic demands accelerate.20,21 Reading difficulties are prevalent, with up to 17% of US children meeting criteria for dyslexia22 and 64% of US fourth-graders scoring below “proficient” in a recent assessment.23,24 Contrary to a prevailing “wait to fail” approach, there is evidence that deficits may be detectable well before formal instruction,25,27 which suggests benefits of early screening and intervention.22,28,30 

Bright Futures screening guidelines do not currently consider emergent literacy as a distinct developmental domain,5 and there is no evidence for linking general surveillance to reading outcomes. Early and/or emergent literacy screening tools for preschool-aged children via caregiver report have recently been developed,31,33 but they are prone to social desirability bias, involve complex concepts and reading levels, lack ecological context, and have either not been validated or are not reliable regarding children <4 years of age. Currently available screeners directly administered to the child34,35 are relatively long and impractical for pediatric practice. Our objective in this study was to develop and provide initial validation of The Reading House (TRH), a children’s book designed to assess emergent literacy skills in 3- and 4-year-old children during well-child visits. Our aims were to establish internal consistency and criterion-related validity and explore TRH’s feasibility and utility as a clinical and teaching tool from the perspective of providers and families, complementing AAP recommendations and existing reading programs.9 

TRH is a full-color, board-format book for young children, measures 6 × 6 in, and is 14 pages long. It has a simple, rhyming narrative (Flesch-Kincaid estimated kindergarten level) and illustrated content playfully modeling early reading skills as children of diverse ethnicity and sexes go about their day (Fig 1). Pediatrician-advocated behaviors such as wearing a bicycle helmet and healthy eating are also reinforced. Before publication, draft versions were pilot tested and psychometrically revised.34 

FIGURE 1

Front cover and sample interior content. The front cover of the book, which the child is asked to identify, is at the top left. At the top right is a half-page spread used to assess alphabet knowledge. The spread at the bottom is used to assess rhyming ability, an early phonological skill.

FIGURE 1

Front cover and sample interior content. The front cover of the book, which the child is asked to identify, is at the top left. At the top right is a half-page spread used to assess alphabet knowledge. The spread at the bottom is used to assess rhyming ability, an early phonological skill.

Close modal

The TRH assessment follows, yet is distinct from, the book’s narrative and is intended to be administered by a clinical provider. Content was developed on the basis of a conceptual model of literacy skills expected to emerge between 3 and 4 years of age14,36: print concepts and knowledge (15%),37 alphabet and letter sound knowledge (29%),38,40 phonological awareness (rhyming, alliteration, syllable blending; 21%),41,42 expressive language and vocabulary (21%),36 and emergent writing (14%).43 The scripted scoring form has 9 items corresponding to the covers and 7 interior page spreads. Wording was refined in consultation with experts in measure development and pilot tested with families and is estimated at a Flesch-Kincaid kindergarten reading level. Administration and scoring are summarized in Table 1.44 

TABLE 1

TRH Content, Administration, and Scoring

SectionEmergent SkillItem ContentPoints
Front cover Concepts of print The child is asked to identify the front cover of the book. 0–1 
Spread 1 Concepts of print Den: 4 bookshelves holding an array of objects; identify 1 with words on it. 0–1 
Spread 2 Alphabet knowledge Garden: all 26 letters are present; “Name as many as you know” (≤8 correct; stop with 2 in a row incorrect). 0–2 
Spread 3 Letter sound knowledge Park: birds that are holding various letters. What sounds do these make (M, B, P)? 0–2 
Spread 4 Phonological awareness (alliteration) Playground: presented with 2 sounds (/s/ and /r/). Which of 3 monosyllabic word objects begin with the sound? 0–1 
Spread 5 Phonological awareness (rhyming) Playroom: Which of 4 monosyllabic word objects rhyme with toys the child is playing with (blocks, truck)? 0–1 
Spread 6 Phonological awareness (syllable blending) Kitchen: Which items on a table result from putting 2 syllable sounds together (wa-ter, ap-ple)? 0–1 
Spread 7 Expressive vocabulary Bedroom: picture naming involving 10 items in the room of variable difficulty. 0–3 
Back cover Emergent writing The child is asked to write his or her name: “This book belongs to.” 0–2 
SectionEmergent SkillItem ContentPoints
Front cover Concepts of print The child is asked to identify the front cover of the book. 0–1 
Spread 1 Concepts of print Den: 4 bookshelves holding an array of objects; identify 1 with words on it. 0–1 
Spread 2 Alphabet knowledge Garden: all 26 letters are present; “Name as many as you know” (≤8 correct; stop with 2 in a row incorrect). 0–2 
Spread 3 Letter sound knowledge Park: birds that are holding various letters. What sounds do these make (M, B, P)? 0–2 
Spread 4 Phonological awareness (alliteration) Playground: presented with 2 sounds (/s/ and /r/). Which of 3 monosyllabic word objects begin with the sound? 0–1 
Spread 5 Phonological awareness (rhyming) Playroom: Which of 4 monosyllabic word objects rhyme with toys the child is playing with (blocks, truck)? 0–1 
Spread 6 Phonological awareness (syllable blending) Kitchen: Which items on a table result from putting 2 syllable sounds together (wa-ter, ap-ple)? 0–1 
Spread 7 Expressive vocabulary Bedroom: picture naming involving 10 items in the room of variable difficulty. 0–3 
Back cover Emergent writing The child is asked to write his or her name: “This book belongs to.” 0–2 

The front and back covers and each of the 7 interior page spreads are used to assess an emergent literacy component skill expected for age. A scripted administration form mirrors but is distinct from the book’s narrative. Points are awarded for correct responses, and the total possible score is 14 points.

The Get Ready to Read! (GRTR) measure was our criterion-referenced standard. GRTR consists of 25 items in tabletop, monochrome format, with an estimated administration time of 10 to 15 minutes. GRTR is validated for ages 3 to 6 years, intended for use by early childhood educators.34 Component skills assessed are print knowledge (25%), alphabet and letter sound knowledge (25%), phonological awareness (rhyming, alliteration, syllable blending, elision; 35%), and emergent writing (15%). GRTR total scores are normalized for age and performance level,45 and the GRTR has been shown to predict reading outcomes.46,47 

Healthy children between 3 and 4 years old were recruited at 7 pediatric primary care clinics in a large Midwestern city. Two clinics were affiliated with an academic children’s hospital serving almost exclusively families of lower socioeconomic status (SES) (>90% were eligible for Medicaid; 75% were African American). Five clinics were private practices serving demographically and geographically diverse communities. Eligibility criteria for all children were as follows: (1) gestation of at least 36 weeks, (2) age at screening of 36 to 52 months, (3) no documented history of global developmental delay or neurobehavioral disorder, and (4) native English–speaking custodial parent. Verbal informed consent was obtained from a custodial parent to expedite screening and minimize interference with clinic flow, and families were provided a different children’s book for participation. Our study was approved by our institutional review board.

Clinical research coordinators (CRCs) practiced administration of TRH via role play sessions and were instructed to adhere to screening scripts verbatim. After informed consent was given, CRCs administered TRH and GRTR in examination rooms before or after seeing the pediatric provider but before vaccine administration to be mindful of patient flow. No identifying or sensitive demographic information was collected per institutional review board restrictions for verbal consent. Order of administration was determined via a random assignment list to control for child fatigue. The time of TRH administration was noted, and after screening, the caregivers were asked 3 questions regarding the time required for TRH screening, whether TRH screening helped them to learn more about their child’s early reading skills, and if it influenced plans to read with their child at home. The child was asked if sharing TRH was fun. Clinical providers (physicians, nurses, and medical assistants) were also surveyed regarding whether screening interfered with clinic flow, their impression of feasibility and usefulness of adopting TRH for everyday practice, perceived barriers to the adoption of TRH, and whether they believe that literacy screening belongs in primary care. Families were not allowed to take TRH home at this preliminary stage. Response data were entered into a secure Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap) database.48 

Descriptive statistics were computed for demographic variables for all children. Descriptive statistics were also computed for all TRH variables at item and scale levels. Modern theory Rasch rating scale methods were used for all TRH items,49,50 which were evaluated for smoothness, modality, difficulty, polarity, and sufficiency of density. Model fit was tested for each item to identify any that were markedly influencing scale-level distributions. Bivariate correlations were computed among pairs of items as well as between each item and TRH total score and GRTR total score by using Spearman-ρ correlation coefficients. Preliminary estimates of TRH reliability and validity were then computed, beginning with Cronbach’s coefficient α (αCr) as our measure of reliability, and Spearman-ρ (rρ) correlation coefficients for criterion-related validity between TRH and GRTR total scores. Spearman-ρ coefficients were also computed between TRH total score and selected demographic variables: child age and sex and clinic type (hospital-affiliated versus private practice). Spearman-ρ correlations were deemed as most appropriate given the non-normal nature of distributions for both TRH and GRTR scores. Ordinal logistic regression models were used to estimate the relationship between GRTR (below average, average, and above average) and TRH scores, controlling for age, sex, and clinic type, respectively, to identify the best-fitting model. The criterion for statistical significance was the unadjusted α = .05 level. Finally, descriptive statistics were obtained for responses to acceptance, feasibility, and usability questions administered to children, parents, and providers across clinics. Statistics for TRH administration time were also determined. All analyses were conducted by using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, NC) and Winsteps version 4.0 software (Rasch Measurement Analysis, Beaverton, OR).

A total of 278 children were screened. Ages ranged from 36 to 52 months (mean: 43.1 ± 5.6 months; 58% were 36–47 months; 42% were 48–52 months), with 125 boys and 153 girls. A total of 109 children (39%) were from hospital-affiliated clinics, and 169 were from private practices (61%).

TRH item–level information is provided in Table 2. Rasch estimates of item difficulty ranged from −3.69 (less difficult; vocabulary) to 2.22 (more difficult; rhyming). Item-fit statistics using empirically derived z values were within the traditional ±2 SDs, which suggests that no outliers likely influenced the distributions.51 Point-measure correlations ranged from 0.21 (alliteration) to 0.77 (vocabulary), which suggests a small-to-moderate relationship between each of the items and the entire scale. Bivariate TRH interitem correlation coefficients ranged from rs = −0.02 (blending-alliteration; none) to 0.58 (letter name–letter sounds; large)52 but were generally small between 0.1 and 0.3 (Table 3). The mean TRH score was 4.2 (±2.9; range: 0–14), and mean GRTR was 11.1 (±4.4; range: 1–25). A scatter plot is shown in Fig 2.

TABLE 2

Item Analysis and Summary Statistics for TRH (Rasch Analysis)

ItemMean (SD)DifficultySEInfit zOutfit zPoint-Measure Correlation
Rhyming 0.1 (0.3) 2.22 0.25 −0.5 0.5 0.25 
Alliteration 0.1 (0.3) 1.42 0.19 0.3 2.0 0.21 
Identify words 0.1 (0.4) 1.32 0.18 0.0 0.3 0.33 
Write name 0.3 (0.6) 0.08 0.13 −0.6 −0.5 0.58 
Letter sounds 0.3 (0.6) 0.08 0.13 −0.4 −0.9 0.62 
Identify cover 0.4 (0.5) −0.05 0.13 0.3 1.5 0.36 
Blending 0.5 (0.5) −0.41 0.12 −2.1 −0.1 0.50 
Letter names 0.6 (0.8) −0.97 0.11 −0.9 −0.9 0.71 
Vocabulary 1.7 (1.0) −3.69 0.09 −1.4 −0.7 0.77 
ItemMean (SD)DifficultySEInfit zOutfit zPoint-Measure Correlation
Rhyming 0.1 (0.3) 2.22 0.25 −0.5 0.5 0.25 
Alliteration 0.1 (0.3) 1.42 0.19 0.3 2.0 0.21 
Identify words 0.1 (0.4) 1.32 0.18 0.0 0.3 0.33 
Write name 0.3 (0.6) 0.08 0.13 −0.6 −0.5 0.58 
Letter sounds 0.3 (0.6) 0.08 0.13 −0.4 −0.9 0.62 
Identify cover 0.4 (0.5) −0.05 0.13 0.3 1.5 0.36 
Blending 0.5 (0.5) −0.41 0.12 −2.1 −0.1 0.50 
Letter names 0.6 (0.8) −0.97 0.11 −0.9 −0.9 0.71 
Vocabulary 1.7 (1.0) −3.69 0.09 −1.4 −0.7 0.77 

Terms used to describe items are summaries of the concepts of the items themselves.

TABLE 3

Intercorrelation Table for TRH Items, TRH Total Score, and GRTR Total Score

Identify WordsLetter NameLetter Sounds Alliteration Rhyming Blending Vocabulary Write Name TRH Total ScoreGRTR Total Score
Identify cover 0.17 0.06 0.10 0.14 0.06 0.11 0.06 0.36 0.35 0.29 
Identify words  0.16 0.13 0.02 0.21 0.11 0.16 0.24 0.33 0.30 
Letter name   0.58 0.08 0.14 0.27 0.53 0.38 0.74 0.57 
Letter sounds    0.15 0.16 0.27 0.44 0.34 0.64 0.46 
Alliteration     0.15 −0.02 −0.01 0.19 0.19 0.17 
Rhyming      0.06 0.13 0.16 0.23 0.22 
Blending       0.23 0.25 0.49 0.27 
Vocabulary        0.29 0.76 0.41 
Write name         0.61 0.54 
Identify WordsLetter NameLetter Sounds Alliteration Rhyming Blending Vocabulary Write Name TRH Total ScoreGRTR Total Score
Identify cover 0.17 0.06 0.10 0.14 0.06 0.11 0.06 0.36 0.35 0.29 
Identify words  0.16 0.13 0.02 0.21 0.11 0.16 0.24 0.33 0.30 
Letter name   0.58 0.08 0.14 0.27 0.53 0.38 0.74 0.57 
Letter sounds    0.15 0.16 0.27 0.44 0.34 0.64 0.46 
Alliteration     0.15 −0.02 −0.01 0.19 0.19 0.17 
Rhyming      0.06 0.13 0.16 0.23 0.22 
Blending       0.23 0.25 0.49 0.27 
Vocabulary        0.29 0.76 0.41 
Write name         0.61 0.54 

All correlations presented here were computed by using the Spearman-ρ correlation coefficient.

FIGURE 2

Scatter plot of TRH versus GRTR with total scores color coded by child age (blue = 3–3.9 years; red = ≥4 years). The mean TRH score was 4.2 (±2.9; range: 0–14), and the mean GRTR score was 11.1 (±4.4; range: 1–25).

FIGURE 2

Scatter plot of TRH versus GRTR with total scores color coded by child age (blue = 3–3.9 years; red = ≥4 years). The mean TRH score was 4.2 (±2.9; range: 0–14), and the mean GRTR score was 11.1 (±4.4; range: 1–25).

Close modal

For reliability, internal consistency was estimated at rCo-α = 0.68, considered “good” for new measures.53 For criterion-related validity, the correlation between TRH and GRTR scores was rs = 0.66 (P < .001; high). There were low-to–moderately positive correlations between TRH total scores and female child sex (rs = 0.18), private practice clinic type (rs = 0.31), and child age (rs = 0.34; all P < .001). In the ordinal logistic regression model, TRH total score (odds ratio: 1.7; 95% confidence interval: 1.5–1.9; P < .001) and child age (odds ratio: 0.9; 95% confidence interval: 0.8–0.9; P < .001) were included in the best-fitting model. Child sex and clinic type were not statistically significant.

A total of 72 clinical providers were surveyed (20 hospital affiliated and 52 from private practice), including 18 medical assistants, 11 nurses, and 43 physicians. Most believed that literacy screening belongs in primary care (85% yes, 15% not sure, 0% no). Few reported that TRH interfered with patient flow (93% not at all, 6% somewhat, 1% very much). Most believed that TRH would be feasible to administer (49% yes, 43% not sure, 8% no), clinically useful (67% yes, 31% not sure, 2% no), and useful for families (85% yes, 14% not sure, 1% no). Responses were not significantly different between physician and nonphysician providers, although nominally more physicians rated TRH as clinically useful and nominally more nonphysicians rated TRH as feasible to administer. The most commonly cited potential barrier to adopting TRH in everyday practice was not enough provider time (68%), with 8% noting staffing concerns and 3% noting no reimbursement.

A total of 248 children and 243 parents responded to surveys regarding their impression of TRH screening, summarized in Fig 3. Most parents reported the time required for TRH screening as reasonable (88% just right, 9% a bit much, 3% too much). Most reported that TRH screening helped them learn about their child’s early reading skills (34% very much, 37% somewhat, 29% not at all) and influenced plans to read with their child at home (29% very much, 29% somewhat, 42% no). Responses for the time required were equivalent between clinic types and more favorable for learning about reading skills and influencing plans to read in hospital-affiliated clinics (P < .05; χ2 test). The majority of children rated sharing TRH as fun (65% yes, 23% kind of, 10% no, 2% no response), which was equivalent between clinic types. The mean TRH administration time was 5:25 minutes (±0:55; range: 3:34–8:32) and was not significantly different between 3- and 4-year-olds or between clinic types.

FIGURE 3

Parent and child impressions of screening with TRH. Histograms of responses to surveys administered to parents (n = 243) and children (n = 248) after screening with TRH during the well-child visit are shown.

FIGURE 3

Parent and child impressions of screening with TRH. Histograms of responses to surveys administered to parents (n = 243) and children (n = 248) after screening with TRH during the well-child visit are shown.

Close modal

Recent insights into early brain development suggest that potential deficits in emergent literacy skills can be identified and addressed proactively well before kindergarten.25,54,55 Concerns with screeners reliant on parent report10,32,33 include susceptibility to social desirability bias, use of complex language and concepts, low reliability at <4 years of age, and, importantly, nonengagement of the child. This is a particular concern in families of low-SES backgrounds who may have fewer opportunities to share books with their child and accurately gauge their abilities. Although parent report is widely used and appealing for practical reasons,56,58 direct screening is ideal. Our objective was to develop and validate a children’s book designed to directly screen emergent literacy skills during well-child visits as early and enjoyably as possible, providing a potential catalyst for anticipatory guidance that complements existing reading programs.9 

The internal psychometric properties of TRH were strong, particularly for a novel measure attempting to capture a dynamic developmental construct with multiple components. We attribute this performance to an evidence-based conceptual model of emergent literacy used to guide development.14,15,36 TRH items exhibited a good range of difficulty, with phonological items (rhyming, alliteration) scoring most difficult and vocabulary and letter naming the easiest (Table 2), consistent with expected trajectories of these skills.36 The relative ease of the letter naming item may reflect its open-ended nature, providing for differences in how children learn letters (eg, those in their own name).40,59,60 The ease of syllable blending relative to the other phonological items may be attributable to the manipulation of larger sound units61 and/or pictures of blended words that are visual cues for the child. By contrast, the rhyming and alliteration items have multiple visually appealing choices, and alliteration requires the processing of smaller sound units.61 Despite word selection from an age-normalized list,62 difficulty of the vocabulary item was exceedingly low (<−3 SD units). We plan to incorporate more difficult word objects into a revised version.

TRH items revealed small-to-moderate interitem correlations, which suggests that each item contributed uniquely to the total score. All correlations were positive, consistent with variable skill trajectories, which emerge in parallel before their integration,36 with some lagging because of neurobiological constraints.17,63 Letter naming and letter sound knowledge were highly correlated with each other and with vocabulary, consistent with previous research.40,64 Interestingly, rhyming and alliteration were weakly correlated, possibly attributable to a high variability of phonological abilities at younger ages36 and likely contributing to relatively weak correlation between these items and total TRH score. Weak correlation between these items and syllable blending may be attributable to relative ease of larger sound unit blending at this age.61 Name writing was positively correlated with almost all other items, consistent with evidence of broad interrelationship of this skill with other emergent skills.65,66 

TRH total scores were positively correlated with age, female sex, and private practice clinic type (our predetermined proxy for SES), consistent with the expected influence of these factors.14,67,68 TRH was strongly correlated with GRTR (concurrent validity), which has been shown to predict reading outcomes and risk for reading difficulties.34,46 This relationship remained statistically significant in our logistic regression model after controlling for age, sex, and clinic type. This robust relationship is unsurprising because TRH components mirror GRTR; however, in addition to format, there are important content differences. Print concepts and emergent writing are ecologically assessed in TRH by inviting the child to identify the front cover and write his or her name on the back. To assess alphabet knowledge, the child is invited to identify letters that they know rather than a prescribed set. TRH is used to assess alliteration as its most advanced phonological skill because we considered elision to be prohibitively difficult for our target age. TRH includes assessment of expressive vocabulary, the earliest emergent skill,36,37 with less weighting of phonological skills. Despite incorporating easier items, TRH scores skewed low, with only 1 child achieving a maximum score and 12 receiving a score of 0. We believe that this reflects the relative difficulty of TRH fueled by its ecological format and minimal use of multiple-choice items (2 of 9 items, each involving 2 trials requiring correct responses to receive credit) to reduce the influence of prompts and guessing. Refinement of future versions may involve the revision of items to make them easier for our intended age range or alternate forms for younger and older children.

Although psychometric properties are critical, effective screening should be perceived as useful and not burdensome or invasive. Responses to parent, child, and provider surveys were favorable, which suggests that TRH screening may be an enjoyable and valuable addition to well-child visits. This seems particularly appealing for families of lower SES, who reported learning more about their child’s skills and a greater influence on plans to read at home. An administration time of ∼5 minutes seems to be a reasonable investment if staff and families view time spent as worthwhile, as suggested in our data. This appeal should be even greater if and/or when reimbursement for literacy screening is implemented and providers are able to use TRH as a teaching tool. The 3-year-old well-child visit may be ideal for TRH administration because vaccines are not typically given, and guidance can be provided proximal to preschool entry.5 Beyond skill assessment, an appealing aspect of TRH involves modeling of reading and other positive health messages in the narrative. Children’s books for anticipatory guidance have been proven effective for this purpose.69,71 Inviting the child to write his or her name on the book exemplifies this dual function of assessing a core skill while providing a sense of ownership and empowerment for families.

This study has limitations. Although construct validity was established, its cross-sectional nature cannot be used to establish predictive validity, which would require a longitudinal design. Because of feasibility constraints, we did not explore test-retest reliability, although this is planned. To expedite screening in busy clinical practices, we collected limited demographic information. However, this served the aims of this study, and clinic type is a reasonable proxy for SES given the population served by our hospital-based clinics. TRH was administered by CRCs, and it is possible that the results would not generalize to clinical providers, although at this preliminary stage, fidelity with administration was paramount and provider impression was surveyed. Indirect assessment of feasibility and utility was likely a major driver of the sizable percentage of “not sure” responses for these items, which we believe would skew favorably in actual practice given the appeal of book distribution and discussion of reading documented in the Reach Out and Read program.9,72 Similarly, we suspect that more parents may have rated TRH as useful if it was administered by providers1,72 rather than CRCs trained to not discuss results or give them the book to take home. GRTR was our sole external criterion,34,47 and exploring relationships between TRH and other standardized measures would be useful. Although it revealed solid internal consistency and reliability, TRH was administered to a broad age range when skills evolve rapidly. In addition to determination of risk strata for older and younger age ranges, development of alternate forms for 3- and 4-year-old children may be useful.

Our study also has important strengths. Our sample was large and well characterized, drawn from diverse practice sites. Screening was conducted during normal pediatric practice by using a book that is scalable and low cost within existing programs such as Reach Out and Read.72 Analyses involved rigorous psychometric methods49,51 and an external criterion shown to predict reading outcomes. Feasibility was explored via large samples of providers, parents, and children. A future study involving TRH administration by clinical providers will allow the exploration of important questions, including optimal age of administration, integration into clinic workflows, and use of TRH for anticipatory guidance. We are developing training materials to encourage fidelity.44 We have also generated estimates of risk thresholds for TRH corresponding to performance categories in the GRTR (below average, average, above average) for children aged 36 to 47 months and 48 to 52 months. Although we will provide these to those interested in using TRH with caveats of their preliminary nature, longitudinal studies are needed to determine predictive validity. Altogether, TRH reveals early promise as a valid and enjoyable means to directly screen emergent literacy skills in 3- and 4-year-old children with potential as a catalyst for reading guidance consistent with AAP screening and literacy recommendations.1,5 

In this preliminary validation study involving emergent literacy screening in 3- and 4-year-old children during well-child visits, TRH revealed promising psychometric properties, including internal consistency and validity referenced to an established standard. Provider, parent, and child impressions of screening in terms of time, enjoyment, and utility were favorable, and administration time was feasible for primary care practice. The children’s book format of TRH also suggests potential as a catalyst for provider-parent reading guidance and is scalable within existing programs. Longitudinal studies are needed to refine risk strata for different age ranges, improve items, assess predictive validity, and test performance when administered by clinical providers.

Dr Hutton conceived, wrote, and coordinated all aspects of the publication of The Reading House book, developed and collaborated in the refinement of the screening instrument, designed all aspects of the study, collaborated in analyses, and drafted the initial manuscript and subsequent revisions; Dr Justice advised Dr Hutton on the design of The Reading House book, the screening instrument, and subsequent revisions and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Mr Huang conducted all statistical analyses, created all figures and tables, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Ms Kerr consulted on the study design, coordinated data collection and screener administration, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr DeWitt provided guidance on the study design, analyses, and clinical application and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Ittenbach directed and supervised psychometric and other data analyses for this study, consulted in the refinement of The Reading House screening instrument, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted.

FUNDING: Funded by a Procter Scholar Award from the Cincinnati Children’s Research Foundation (Dr Hutton).

We thank Arielle Wilson for her diligence in collecting data and the providers at the 7 clinical sites, Pediatric Primary Care Clinic, Hopple Street Clinic, Springdale-Mason Pediatrics, Pediatricians of Hyde Park, Northeast Pediatrics, Group Health Associates: Western Hills, and Pediatric Associates of Mount Carmel, who graciously hosted this research. We also thank the Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center Research Foundation for their support of early-career investigators and this work. For more information about TRH or to order it for research or clinical use, visit http://www.bluemanateepress.com/nonprofit/ or e-mail press@bluemanateebooks.com or john1.hutton@cchmc.org.

AAP

American Academy of Pediatrics

CRC

clinical research coordinator

GRTR

Get Ready to Read!

SES

socioeconomic status

TRH

The Reading House

1
High
PC
,
Klass
P
;
Council on Early Childhood
.
Literacy promotion: an essential component of primary care pediatric practice.
Pediatrics
.
2014
;
134
(
2
):
404
409
[PubMed]
2
Donoghue
EA
;
Council on Early Childhood
.
Quality early education and child care from birth to kindergarten.
Pediatrics
.
2017
;
140
(
2
):
e20171488
[PubMed]
3
Horowitz
SH
,
Rawe
J
,
Whittaker
MC
.
The State of Learning Disabilities: Understanding the 1 in 5
.
New York, NY
:
National Center for Learning Disabilities
;
2017
4
Simon
GR
,
Baker
C
,
Barden
GA
 III
, et al;
Committee on Practice and Ambulatory Medicine
;
Bright Futures Periodicity Schedule Workgroup
.
2014 recommendations for pediatric preventive health care.
Pediatrics
.
2014
;
133
(
3
):
568
570
[PubMed]
5
American Academy of Pediatrics Bright Futures National Center
.
Bright Futures: Guidelines for Health Supervision of Infants, Children, and Adolescents (4th edition)
. 3rd ed.
Elk Grove Village, IL
:
American Academy of Pediatrics
;
2017
6
D’Souza
S
,
Waldie
KE
,
Peterson
ER
,
Underwood
L
,
Morton
SM
.
Psychometric properties and normative data for the preschool Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in two-year-old children.
J Abnorm Child Psychol
.
2017
;
45
(
2
):
345
357
[PubMed]
7
Fothergill
KE
,
Gadomski
A
,
Solomon
BS
, et al
.
Assessing the impact of a web-based comprehensive somatic and mental health screening tool in pediatric primary care.
Acad Pediatr
.
2013
;
13
(
4
):
340
347
[PubMed]
8
Epstein
JN
,
Kelleher
KJ
,
Baum
R
, et al
.
Impact of a web-portal intervention on community ADHD care and outcomes.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
2
):
e20154240
[PubMed]
9
Zuckerman
B
,
Khandekar
A
.
Reach Out and Read: evidence based approach to promoting early child development.
Curr Opin Pediatr
.
2010
;
22
(
4
):
539
544
[PubMed]
10
Iyer
SN
,
Dawson
MZ
,
Sawyer
MI
,
Abdullah
N
,
Saju
L
,
Needlman
RD
.
Added value of early literacy screening in preschool children.
Clin Pediatr (Phila)
.
2017
;
56
(
10
):
959
963
[PubMed]
11
Kolb
B
,
Harker
A
,
Gibb
R
.
Principles of plasticity in the developing brain.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2017
;
59
(
12
):
1218
1223
[PubMed]
12
Knudsen
EI
.
Sensitive periods in the development of the brain and behavior.
J Cogn Neurosci
.
2004
;
16
(
8
):
1412
1425
[PubMed]
13
Petrican
R
,
Taylor
MJ
,
Grady
CL
.
Trajectories of brain system maturation from childhood to older adulthood: implications for lifespan cognitive functioning.
Neuroimage
.
2017
;
163
:
125
149
[PubMed]
14
Whitehurst
GJ
,
Lonigan
CJ
.
Child development and emergent literacy.
Child Dev
.
1998
;
69
(
3
):
848
872
[PubMed]
15
Rohde
L
.
The comprehensive emergent literacy model: early literacy in context.
SAGE Open
.
2015
;
5
(
1
):
1
11
16
Foorman
BR
,
Anthony
J
,
Seals
L
,
Mouzaki
A
.
Language development and emergent literacy in preschool.
Semin Pediatr Neurol
.
2002
;
9
(
3
):
173
184
[PubMed]
17
Horowitz-Kraus
T
,
Hutton
JS
.
From emergent literacy to reading: how learning to read changes a child’s brain.
Acta Paediatr
.
2015
;
104
(
7
):
648
656
18
National Center for Education Statistics
.
Early Childhood Longitudinal Program Birth Cohort (ECLS-B)
.
Washington, DC
:
US Department of Education
;
2011
19
Center on Children and Families at Brookings
.
Starting School at a Disadvantage: The School Readiness of Poor Children
.
Washington, DC
:
Brookings Institution
;
2012
20
Gabrieli
JD
.
Dyslexia: a new synergy between education and cognitive neuroscience.
Science
.
2009
;
325
(
5938
):
280
283
[PubMed]
21
The Annie E. Casey Foundation
.
Double Jeopardy: How Third Grade Reading Skills and Poverty Influence High School Graduation
.
Baltimore, MD
:
The Annie E. Casey Foundation
;
2012
22
Ozernov-Palchik
O
,
Norton
ES
,
Sideridis
G
, et al
.
Longitudinal stability of pre-reading skill profiles of kindergarten children: implications for early screening and theories of reading.
Dev Sci
.
2017
;
20
(
5
):
e12471
[PubMed]
23
National Center for Education Statistics
.
National Assessment of Educational Progress at Grades 4 and 8: Mathematics and Reading
.
Washington, DC
:
National Center for Education Statistics
;
2015
24
Bock
R
.
Reading Difficulty and Disability: Fact Sheet
.
Washington, DC
:
National Institutes of Health
;
2010
25
Gaab
N
.
It’s a Myth That Young Children Cannot Be Screened for Dyslexia!
Baltimore, MD
:
International Dyslexia Association
;
2017
. Available at: https://dyslexiaida.org/its-a-myth-that-young-children-cannot-be-screened-for-dyslexia/. Accessed April, 2018
26
Leppänen
PH
,
Hämäläinen
JA
,
Guttorm
TK
, et al
.
Infant brain responses associated with reading-related skills before school and at school age.
Neurophysiol Clin
.
2012
;
42
(
1–2
):
35
41
[PubMed]
27
Puolakanaho
A
,
Ahonen
T
,
Aro
M
, et al
.
Developmental links of very early phonological and language skills to second grade reading outcomes: strong to accuracy but only minor to fluency.
J Learn Disabil
.
2008
;
41
(
4
):
353
370
[PubMed]
28
Pool
JL
,
Johnson
ES
.
Screening for Reading Problems in Preschool and Kindergarten: An Overview of Select Measures
.
Boise, ID
:
Boise State University
;
2016
29
Blachman
BA
, ed.
Foundations of Reading Acquisition and Dyslexia: Implications for Early Intervention
.
Mahwah, NJ
:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers
;
1997
30
Snow
CE
,
Burns
MS
,
Griffin
P
.
Preventing Reading Difficulties in Young Children
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academy Press
;
1998
31
Iyer
SN
,
Sawyer
MI
,
Lambert
G
,
Hartwell
A
,
Needlman
RD
.
Validation of a five-item parent questionnaire to screen preschool children for reading problems.
Clin Pediatr (Phila)
.
2016
;
55
(
5
):
492
495
[PubMed]
32
Bailet
LL
,
Zettler-Greeley
C
,
Lewis
K
.
Psychometric profile of an experimental Emergent Literacy Screener for preschoolers.
Sch Psychol Q
.
2018
;
33
(
1
):
120
136
33
Nemours BrightStart!
Preschool reading screener. 2018. Available at: www.readingbrightstart.org/reading-screener/. Accessed January 1, 2019
34
Lonigan
C
,
Wilson
S
.
Report on the Revised Get Ready to Read! Screening Tool: Psychometrics and Normative Information
.
New York, NY
:
National Center on Learning Disabilities
;
2008
35
Kaminski
RA
,
Abbott
M
,
Bravo Aguayo
K
,
Latimer
R
,
Good
RH
 III
.
The preschool early literacy indicators: validity and benchmark goals.
Top Early Child Spec Educ
.
2014
;
34
(
2
):
71
82
36
Horowitz-Kraus
T
,
Schmitz
R
,
Hutton
JS
,
Schumacher
J
.
How to create a successful reader? Milestones in reading development from birth to adolescence.
Acta Paediatr
.
2017
;
106
(
4
):
534
544
37
Clay
MM
.
Concepts About Print: What Have Children Learned About Printed Language?
Portsmouth, NH
:
Heinemann
;
2000
38
Worden
PE
,
Boettcher
W
.
Young children’s acquisition of alphabet knowledge.
J Read Behav
.
1990
;
22
(
3
):
277
295
39
Piasta
SB
,
Petscher
Y
,
Justice
LM
.
How many letters should preschoolers in public programs know? The diagnostic efficiency of various preschool letter-naming benchmarks for predicting first-grade literacy achievement.
J Educ Psychol
.
2012
;
104
(
4
):
954
958
[PubMed]
40
Justice
LM
,
Pence
K
,
Bowles
RB
,
Wiggins
A
.
An investigation of four hypotheses concerning the order by which 4-year-old children learn the alphabet letters.
Early Child Res Q
.
2006
;
21
(
3
):
374
389
41
Snowling
M
,
Hulme
C
.
The development of phonological skills.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
.
1994
;
346
(
1315
):
21
27
[PubMed]
42
Melby-Lervåg
M
,
Lyster
SA
,
Hulme
C
.
Phonological skills and their role in learning to read: a meta-analytic review.
Psychol Bull
.
2012
;
138
(
2
):
322
352
[PubMed]
43
Cabell
SQ
,
Justice
LM
,
Zucker
TA
,
McGinty
AS
.
Emergent name-writing abilities of preschool-age children with language impairment.
Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch
.
2009
;
40
(
1
):
53
66
[PubMed]
44
Kerr
A
,
Justice
L
,
Huang
G
,
Ittenbach
RF
,
Hutton
JS
.
The Reading House Administration and Technical Manual
.
Cincinnati, OH
:
Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Reading and Literacy Discovery Center
;
2018
45
Good
RH
,
Kaminski
RA
,
Cummings
K
, et al
.
Dynamic Indicators of Basic Early Literacy Skills, Next Edition
.
Longmont, CO
:
Sopris
;
2010
46
Wilson
SB
,
Lonigan
CJ
.
An evaluation of two emergent literacy screening tools for preschool children.
Ann Dyslexia
.
2009
;
59
(
2
):
115
131
[PubMed]
47
Phillips
BM
,
Lonigan
CJ
,
Wyatt
MA
.
Predictive validity of the Get Ready to Read! Screener: concurrent and long-term relations with reading-related skills.
J Learn Disabil
.
2009
;
42
(
2
):
133
147
[PubMed]
48
Harris
PA
,
Taylor
R
,
Thielke
R
,
Payne
J
,
Gonzalez
N
,
Conde
JG
.
Research electronic data capture (REDCap)–a metadata-driven methodology and workflow process for providing translational research informatics support.
J Biomed Inform
.
2009
;
42
(
2
):
377
381
[PubMed]
49
Smith
EV
,
Smith
RM
.
Rasch Measurement: Advanced and Specialized Applications
.
Maple Grove, MN
:
JAM Press
;
2007
50
Rasch
G
.
On general laws and the meaning of measurement in psychology
. In:
Proceedings of the Fourth Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability
;
June 20–July 30, 1960
;
Berkeley, CA
51
Smith
RM
.
Detecting item bias with the Rasch model.
J Appl Meas
.
2004
;
5
(
4
):
430
449
[PubMed]
52
Cohen
P
,
Cohen
P
,
West
S
,
Aiken
L
.
Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences
.
New York, NY
:
Psychology Press
;
1983
53
Peterson
RA
.
A meta-analysis of Cronbach’s coefficient alpha
.
J Consum Res
.
1994
;
21
(
2
):
381
391
54
Shonkoff
JP
,
Boyce
WT
,
McEwen
BS
.
Neuroscience, molecular biology, and the childhood roots of health disparities: building a new framework for health promotion and disease prevention.
JAMA
.
2009
;
301
(
21
):
2252
2259
[PubMed]
55
National Scientific Council on the Developing Child
.
Working Paper #5: The Timing and Quality of Early Experiences Combine to Shape Brain Architecture
.
Cambridge, MA
:
Harvard University Center on the Developing Child
;
2008
56
Veldhuizen
S
,
Clinton
J
,
Rodriguez
C
,
Wade
TJ
,
Cairney
J
.
Concurrent validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires and Bayley Developmental Scales in a general population sample.
Acad Pediatr
.
2015
;
15
(
2
):
231
237
[PubMed]
57
Dreyer
BP
,
Mendelsohn
AL
,
Tamis-LeMonda
CS
.
Assessing the child’s cognitive home environment through parental report: reliability and validity.
Early Dev Parent
.
1996
;
5
:
271
287
58
Hutton
JS
,
Huang
G
,
Phelan
KJ
,
DeWitt
T
,
Ittenbach
RF
.
Shared reading quality assessment by parental report: preliminary validation of the DialogPR.
BMC Pediatr
.
2018
;
18
(
1
):
330
[PubMed]
59
Pence Turnbull
KL
,
Bowles
RP
,
Skibbe
LE
,
Justice
LM
,
Wiggins
AK
.
Theoretical explanations for preschoolers’ lowercase alphabet knowledge.
J Speech Lang Hear Res
.
2010
;
53
(
6
):
1757
1768
[PubMed]
60
Phillips
BM
,
Piasta
SB
,
Anthony
JL
,
Lonigan
CJ
,
Francis
DJ
.
IRTs of the ABCs: children’s letter name acquisition.
J Sch Psychol
.
2012
;
50
(
4
):
461
481
[PubMed]
61
Phillips
BM
,
Menchetti
JC
,
Lonigan
CJ
.
Successful phonological awareness instruction with preschool children: lessons from the classroom.
Top Early Child Spec Educ
.
2008
;
28
(
1
):
3
17
[PubMed]
62
Kuperman
V
,
Stadthagen-Gonzalez
H
,
Brysbaert
M
.
Age-of-acquisition ratings for 30,000 English words.
Behav Res Methods
.
2012
;
44
(
4
):
978
990
[PubMed]
63
Chyl
K
,
Kossowski
B
,
Dębska
A
, et al
.
Prereader to beginning reader: changes induced by reading acquisition in print and speech brain networks.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2018
;
59
(
1
):
76
87
[PubMed]
64
Evans
MA
,
Bell
M
,
Shaw
D
,
Moretti
S
,
Page
J
.
Letter names, letter sounds, and phonological awareness: an examination of kindergarten children across letters and of letters across children.
Read Writ
.
2006
;
19
:
959
989
65
Welsch
JG
,
Sullivan
A
,
Justice
LM
.
That’s my letter!: what preschoolers’ name writing representations tell us about emergent literacy knowledge.
J Literacy Res
.
2003
;
35
(
2
):
757
776
66
Puranik
CS
,
Lonigan
CJ
.
Name-writing proficiency, not length of name, is associated with preschool children’s emergent literacy skills.
Early Child Res Q
.
2012
;
27
(
2
):
284
294
[PubMed]
67
Loveless
T
.
How Well Are American Students Learning? With Sections on the Gender Gap in Reading, Effects of the Common Core, and Student Engagement
.
Washington, DC
:
Brown Center on Education Policy at Brookings
;
2015
68
Noble
KG
,
Farah
MJ
,
McCandliss
BD
.
Socioeconomic background modulates cognition-achievement relationships in reading.
Cogn Dev
.
2006
;
21
(
3
):
349
368
[PubMed]
69
Hutton
JS
,
Gupta
R
,
Gruber
R
, et al
.
Randomized trial of a children’s book versus brochures for safe sleep knowledge and adherence in a high-risk population.
Acad Pediatr
.
2017
;
17
(
8
):
879
886
[PubMed]
70
Eismann
EA
,
Pearl
ES
,
Theuerlinga
J
,
Folger
A
,
Hutton
JS
,
Makoroff
K
.
Calm baby gently program: feasibility of an educational baby book intervention to prevent abusive head trauma within pediatric primary care
. In:
Proceedings From the Ray E. Helfer Society Annual Meeting
,
2017
;
Denver, CO
71
Reich
SM
,
Penner
EK
,
Duncan
GJ
.
Using baby books to increase new mothers’ safety practices.
Acad Pediatr
.
2011
;
11
(
1
):
34
43
[PubMed]
72
Klass
P
,
Dreyer
BP
,
Mendelsohn
AL
.
Reach out and read: literacy promotion in pediatric primary care.
Adv Pediatr
.
2009
;
56
:
11
27
[PubMed]

Competing Interests

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: Dr Hutton conceived, wrote, and edited the children’s book used in this study. Dr Hutton is the founder of blue manatee press, the publisher of the book, although he receives no salary or other compensation for this role. The Reading House is not intended for sale in retail, library, or other direct-to-consumer channels. Its intended use is as a screening tool, distributed at low cost to clinical practices and organizations. Royalties, if any, will be determined via intellectual property policies at Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center; the other authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: Dr Hutton is the founder of blue manatee press, the publisher and distributor of The Reading House, although he receives no salary or other compensation for this role. The Reading House is not intended for sale in retail, library, or other direct-to-consumer channels. Its intended use is as a screening tool, distributed at low cost to clinical practices and organizations. Royalties, if any, will be determined via intellectual property policies at Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center; the other authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.