Video Abstract

Video Abstract

Close modal
OBJECTIVES:

To estimate associations between kindergarten readiness and academic, psychological, and health risks by end of high school.

METHODS:

This study is based on 966 Canadian children. At age 5, trained examiners assessed child number knowledge and receptive vocabulary. Teachers reported kindergarten classroom engagement. At age 17, participants reported academic grades, school connectedness, anxiety sensitivity, substance abuse, physical activity involvement, and height and weight. High school dropout risk was also estimated for each participant on the basis of grades, school engagement, and grade retention.

RESULTS:

Kindergarten math skills contributed to better end-of high school grades (β = .17, P < .01) and lower dropout risk (β = −.20, P < .001), whereas receptive vocabulary predicted lower anxiety sensitivity (β = −.11, P < .01). Kindergarten classroom engagement predicted higher end of high school grades (β = .17, P < .001), lower dropout risk (β = −.20, P < .01), better school connectedness (β = .15, P < .01), lower risk of substance abuse (β = −.21, P < .001), and more physical activity involvement (β = .09, P < .05). Kindergarten classroom engagement was also associated with a 65% reduction (odds ratio = 0.35) in the odds of being overweight at age of 17. Analyses were adjusted for key child (sex, weight per gestational age, nonverbal IQ, and internalizing and externalizing behaviors) and family (parental involvement, maternal depression and immigrant status, family configuration, and socioeconomic status) characteristics.

CONCLUSIONS:

Early childhood readiness forecasts a protective edge by emerging adulthood. With these findings, we build links between education and health indicators, suggesting that children who start school prepared gain a lifestyle advantage. Promoting kindergarten readiness could reduce the health burden generated by high school dropout.

What’s Known on This Subject:

Researchers have shown that being ready to learn in kindergarten can forecast achievement in later elementary school. Furthermore, separate researchers have established a link between high school achievement, lifestyle choices, and productivity.

What This Study Adds:

With our results, we suggest long-term protective associations between readiness to learn in kindergarten and personal success at age 17. With this study, we therefore reaffirm the importance of early childhood readiness and suggest that early interventions could benefit population productivity and health.

Many children begin kindergarten inadequately prepared to benefit from classroom instruction.1,2  Children who are unprepared risk struggling throughout their academic journey.3  Among physical wellbeing, general knowledge, and social emotional wellbeing characteristics, compelling evidence suggests that fundamental skills in math and vocabulary are invariably the most crucial predictors of academic success in later elementary.35  Kindergarten classroom engagement skills, which reflect the ability to successfully adjust to classroom demands, also appear to foster an academic edge throughout elementary school.6  Indeed, productive learning behaviors in kindergarten predict educational attainment by emerging adulthood.7 

In addition to academic success, school readiness has been linked to health and social outcomes. According to researchers of one study, students who began kindergarten with better vocabulary and math skills showed greater chances of healthy lifestyle behavior by the end of fourth grade.8  Kindergarten classroom engagement has also been found to forecast more optimal emotional adjustment, relationships with classmates, participation in physical activity, and BMI by the end of elementary school.6,911 

Educational attainment is closely intertwined with important health, wellbeing, and productivity outcomes for both individuals and society.1214  However, to date, much of the work on school readiness has examined how school readiness contributes to success in later elementary school.35  A longer-term follow-up of school readiness on later indicators of vocational success can therefore afford crucial evidence of the importance of ensuring that all children are ready to learn at the time of school entry. The 20th century expression that all one really needs to know they learned in kindergarten appeals to us for its health, social, and economic implications. A compelling way to evaluate that adage would be to examine the extent to which cognitive and learning skills predict long-term outcomes and indictors of life success.

The objective with this study is to examine the extent to which kindergarten readiness skills, reflecting previous experiences and learning, forecast later mental and physical health risks by the end of high school, an important life course turning point for youth. As a result, we examine academic performance, dropout risk, school connectedness, anxiety sensitivity, substance abuse, physical activity, and healthy weight outcomes by age 17, which marks the end of high school in the province of Quebec. Given that development is presumably influenced by multiple individual and environmental factors across childhood,15,16  we adjust for child sex, birth weight per gestational age, kindergarten nonverbal IQ, and behavior problems as well as parental and family demographic characteristics as potential confounding variables. Furthermore, in regressions addressing links between school readiness and end of high school physical activity and BMI, we control for kindergarten BMI and physical fitness.

We conducted secondary analyses using data from the Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development (QLSCD; 1998–2019). The QLSCD originates from a randomly selected, stratified potential sample of 2837 infants from the birth registry between 1997 and 1998 in Quebec, Canada. From these, 2120 children were retained for longitudinal follow-up from 5 months onward. The retained participants, representing 82% of the eligible target population, are considered representative of all singleton births that took place in the province between 1997 and 1998. Sex was equally represented, with 49% girls and 72% of parents described their child as being Canadian. Most parents (81%) reported that their spoken language was French. Finally, 21.7% of parents reported being under the poverty line cutoff for Canadian families. For complete sample description, see Supplemental Information.

Math skills were assessed by using an abridged version of the Number Knowledge Test (NKT).17  The NKT measures the child’s degree of familiarity with basic notions of arithmetic such as counting, adding, and subtracting, as a function of age. For additional details on this measure see Supplemental Information.

Verbal skills were assessed by using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test,18  which was administered individually in French or English by a trained research assistant. This test comprises practice images, followed by 170 other images in order of increasing difficulty. The starting point depends on the child’s age. Scores are standardized as a function of tables of age-related normative criteria.19  The results of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test are strongly correlated with typical language subscales of most standardized intelligence tests.18 

Classroom engagement was reported by kindergarten teachers using a 7-item scale: works and plays cooperatively with other children, follows rules and instructions, follows directions, listens attentively, completes work on time, works autonomously, and works neatly and carefully (α = .94). Responses were made on a Likert scale and ranged from 1 (never) to 3 (always) for each item. Higher scores reflect better engagement. This classroom engagement measure is empirically related with academic ability.5,6 

Academic Achievement

Academic performance was based on student self-reported average marks in math and English and French language from the previous year. A global academic achievement variable reflecting mean marks in percentage across both disciplines was computed, sample mean (SD) = 75.62% (10.29).

Dropout Risk

Dropout risk was assessed by creating an index comprising academic performance, school engagement, and previous grade retention.

School Engagement

This index has been shown to be a valid predictor of high school dropout by age 25.20  For additional details on this measure, see Supplemental Information.

School Connectedness

School connectedness was based on students answers to the following questions: I am proud I go to this school, I am happy to go to this school, I feel safe at my school, most mornings I look forward to going to school, I like my school (α = .86). Items were rated using the following scale: strongly disagree, disagree, unsure, agree, strongly agree.

Anxiety Sensitivity

Participants indicated the extent to which they agreed with the following statements from the substance use risk profile scale21 : it is frightening to feel dizzy or faint, it frightens me when I feel my heart beat change, I get scared when I am too nervous, I get scared when I experience unusual body sensations (feelings), it scares me when I am unable to focus on a task. All items were rated as either 1 (completely disagree), 2 (disagree), 3 (agree), or 4 (completely agree), α = .82.

Substance Abuse

Substance abuse was based on youth responses of yes (scored as 1) or no (scored as 0) to the following situations over the past 12 months: my alcohol or drug use has had negative psychological effects on me (anxiety, depression, problems concentrating, etc), my alcohol or drug use has had negative effects on my relationships with my family, my alcohol or drug use has had negative effects with my friends or in my love life, my alcohol or drug use has had negative effects on my studies, I committed a delinquent act (even if I was not arrested by the police) while under the influence of alcohol or drugs, I have had the feeling as though the same quantities of alcohol or drugs were having less effect on me than they once had, I have spoken with a health care worker or counselor about my alcohol or drug use. Sums were then converted to scores ranging from 0 to 10.

Physical Activity

Participants answered questions regarding their level of physical activity during their free time. Items included frequency of physical activity during a typical week, the amount of time they spent being physically active on a typical day, their level of effort during physical activities, and the amount of time spent surfing the Internet for school work and leisure, playing video games, watching television shows and movies, and reading for school work. For additional details, see Supplemental Information.

BMI

To derive BMI, participants self-reported weight and height. Weight status was classified as healthy or overweight and/or obese according to International Obesity Task Force cutoffs for BMI ([weight in kg]/height in m2) specific for age and sex.

Child nonverbal IQ was directly assessed by trained professionals when children were in kindergarten by using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children, Third Edition.22  This subtest, which assesses visual perception and organization and abstract reasoning, provides an estimate of nonverbal IQ. Kindergarten teachers reported on child emotional distress and physical aggression using items derived from the Child Behavior Checklist and Preschool Behavior Questionnaire.2325  Finally, weight for gestational age, child age, and sex were derived from medical records. Parental involvement was measured by using the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment, Infant version.26  Trained examiners made assessments after observing parent–child interactions for a period of 3 hours. The following 5 items were assessed: provides toys that challenge child to develop new skills, structures child’s play periods, interacts with her child while engaged in other tasks, encourages her child’s progress, and values educational toys (α = .85). These items were rated as 1 (never), 2 (rarely), 3 (sometimes), 4 (often), or 5 (always). When children were 5 months, mothers also self-reported depressive symptoms using the modified lifetime depression section of the Diagnostic Interview Scale, and family configuration (2 parent = 1 and not = 0) and maternal immigration status (nonimmigrant or European immigrant = 0, non-European immigrant = 1). An index of socioeconomic status was derived from mother and father reports of income, level of education, and occupational prestige. Scores were standardized to a mean of 0. Details on this method have been published elsewhere.26,27 

Our aim was to estimate prospective associations between kindergarten readiness skills (age 5) and educational and health outcomes by the end of high school (age 17). A series of multiple regressions were used to simultaneously estimate the respective and unique contribution of kindergarten number knowledge, receptive vocabulary, and classroom engagement to achievement, dropout risk, school connectedness, anxiety sensitivity, substance abuse, and physical activity. Logistic regressions were used to estimate associations between school readiness characteristics and odds of being overweight. Analyses were adjusted for key child and family contextual characteristics.

In Tables 1 and 2, we provide descriptive statistics on continuous and categorical variables. Boys scored significantly lower on classroom engagement than girls (mean = 2.76 vs 2.60, P < .001); however, there were no sex-based differences in number knowledge or receptive vocabulary scores. Boys also had lower academic averages at the end of high school (mean = 68. 35 vs 74.38, P < .001).

TABLE 1

Descriptive Statistics for Continuous Variables

Mean (SD)Minimum to Maximumn
Predictors    
 No. knowledge 13.26 (3.27) 3 to 18 949 
 Receptive vocabulary 80.38 (17.20) 12 to 130 931 
 Classroom engagement 2.68 (0.37) 1.14 to 3.00 966 
Outcomes    
 Global achievement 76.28 (9.71) 44.5 to 98.5 671 
 Dropout risk .24 (.25) .01 to .99 670 
 School engagement 13.87 (2.40) 5.00 to 18.00 673 
 Anxiety sensitivity 3.32 (2.38) 0 to 10.0 746 
 Substance abuse 0.75 (1.17) 0 to 10 641 
 Physical activity 3.12 (1.55) 1 to 5 730 
Control variables (kindergarten)    
 Nonverbal IQ 19.86 (10.02) 0 to 52.0 958 
 Emotional distress 1.68 (1.64) 0 to 8.33 965 
 Physical aggression 0.95 (2.04) 0 to 10.0 964 
 Child BMI 15.80 (2.03) 11.10 to 31.00 937 
 Child fitness (sit up test) 7.05 (4.79) 0 to 24 880 
Control variables (preschool)    
 Parental involvement 4.90 (2.33) 0 to 10.0 854 
 Maternal depression 1.34 (1.30) 0 to 9.23 964 
 Socioeconomic status 0.09 (0.96) −3.01 to 2.82 965 
Mean (SD)Minimum to Maximumn
Predictors    
 No. knowledge 13.26 (3.27) 3 to 18 949 
 Receptive vocabulary 80.38 (17.20) 12 to 130 931 
 Classroom engagement 2.68 (0.37) 1.14 to 3.00 966 
Outcomes    
 Global achievement 76.28 (9.71) 44.5 to 98.5 671 
 Dropout risk .24 (.25) .01 to .99 670 
 School engagement 13.87 (2.40) 5.00 to 18.00 673 
 Anxiety sensitivity 3.32 (2.38) 0 to 10.0 746 
 Substance abuse 0.75 (1.17) 0 to 10 641 
 Physical activity 3.12 (1.55) 1 to 5 730 
Control variables (kindergarten)    
 Nonverbal IQ 19.86 (10.02) 0 to 52.0 958 
 Emotional distress 1.68 (1.64) 0 to 8.33 965 
 Physical aggression 0.95 (2.04) 0 to 10.0 964 
 Child BMI 15.80 (2.03) 11.10 to 31.00 937 
 Child fitness (sit up test) 7.05 (4.79) 0 to 24 880 
Control variables (preschool)    
 Parental involvement 4.90 (2.33) 0 to 10.0 854 
 Maternal depression 1.34 (1.30) 0 to 9.23 964 
 Socioeconomic status 0.09 (0.96) −3.01 to 2.82 965 

—, not applicable.

TABLE 2

Frequency Distribution for Categorical Variables

Percentage of Samplen
BMI (age 17)  719 
 Overweight 22.4 161 
Sex (5 mo)  966 
 Boys 46.8 452 
Birth weight  965 
 Low birth weight 4.0 39 
Immigrant (5 mo)   
 Non-European immigrant 5.1 966 
Family configuration (5 mo)  965 
 Nonintact 23.3 225 
Percentage of Samplen
BMI (age 17)  719 
 Overweight 22.4 161 
Sex (5 mo)  966 
 Boys 46.8 452 
Birth weight  965 
 Low birth weight 4.0 39 
Immigrant (5 mo)   
 Non-European immigrant 5.1 966 
Family configuration (5 mo)  965 
 Nonintact 23.3 225 

Given that it was possible to estimate incomplete data on outcome variables based on covariates, we conducted multiple imputation on outcome and control variables, assuming that data were missing at random.28  All regression analyses are based on pooled imputed data. A comparison of children with and without kindergarten reported data is available in Supplemental Information.

Adjusted linear and logistic regression results are documented in Tables 3 and 4, respectively. To facilitate effect size comparisons across measures, we report standardized regression coefficients in Table 3. After holding all other variables constant, a 1-point increase on the number knowledge assessment corresponded to 0.518-U increase in academic grades (P = .001) and a 0.016-U decrease in high school dropout risk (P = .001). Each 1-point increase in kindergarten vocabulary scores was associated with 0.016-U decreases on our measure of anxiety (P = .001). Finally, a 1-point increase in classroom engagement scores predicted a 4.66-U increase in academic grades (P = .007), a 0.872-U increase in school connectedness (P = .005), a 0.406-U increase in physical activity (P = .047), a 0.138-U decrease in dropout risk (P = .002), and a 1.01-U decrease in substance abuse scores (P < .001). In terms of the logistic regression, each 1-point increase in classroom engagement scores was associated with a 65% reduction in the odds of being overweight (odds ratio = 0.35, 95% confidence interval, 0.16 to 0.74; P = .009). To interpret these results more concretely, increasing classroom engagement scores by 1 point was associated with an additional 4.66 percentage points in academic grades as well as a full-scale point decrease in substance abuse risk, which can both be interpreted as clinically significant effects. To further contextualize these results, we found small to moderate effect sizes for models predicting academic achievement and dropout, explaining 30% and 35% of the variance, whereas models predicting anxiety (10%), physical activity (10%), substance abuse (6%), and school connectedness (5%) were smaller.

TABLE 3

Adjusted Standardized Regression Coefficients Depicting Associations Between Kindergarten School Readiness Skills and Outcomes at Age 17

Global AchievementDropout RiskSchool ConnectednessAnxietySubstance AbusePhysical Activity
βPβPβPβPβPβP
No. knowledge .17 >.001 −.20 >.001 −.01 .764 .024 .337 .06 .206 .01 .888 
Receptive vocabulary .04 .834 −.03 .162 .06 .264 −.11 .007 −.04 .445 .01 .078 
Classroom engagement .17 .004 −.20 .002 .15 .005 −.08 .098 −.21 >.001 .09 .047 
R2 .30 — .35 — .05 — .10 — .06 — .10 — 
Global AchievementDropout RiskSchool ConnectednessAnxietySubstance AbusePhysical Activity
βPβPβPβPβPβP
No. knowledge .17 >.001 −.20 >.001 −.01 .764 .024 .337 .06 .206 .01 .888 
Receptive vocabulary .04 .834 −.03 .162 .06 .264 −.11 .007 −.04 .445 .01 .078 
Classroom engagement .17 .004 −.20 .002 .15 .005 −.08 .098 −.21 >.001 .09 .047 
R2 .30 — .35 — .05 — .10 — .06 — .10 — 

Models are adjusted for child sex, low birth wt, kindergarten nonverbal IQ, emotional distress and physical aggression, parental involvement, maternal depression and immigrant status, family configuration, and socioeconomic status.

TABLE 4

Adjusted Odds Ratios Depicting Associations Between Kindergarten Classroom Engagement and wt Status at Age 17

Overweight OR (95% CI)P
Number knowledge 1.01 (0.93–1.10) .804 
Receptive vocabulary 1.01 (0.99–1.02) .284 
Classroom engagement 0.35 (0.16–0.76) .010 
Overweight OR (95% CI)P
Number knowledge 1.01 (0.93–1.10) .804 
Receptive vocabulary 1.01 (0.99–1.02) .284 
Classroom engagement 0.35 (0.16–0.76) .010 

Models are adjusted for child sex, low birth wt, kindergarten BMI, fitness, emotional distress and physical aggression, parental involvement, maternal depression and immigrant status, family configuration, and socioeconomic status. CI, confidence interval.

At a time when youth obesity and mental health risks have increased,29,30  with the current study we provide timely evidence that early readiness for school sets the stage for population increases in wellbeing, health, and human capital formation. With these results, it is also suggested that early success begets continued success, which has been previously noted in education research.31,32  Early math skills predicted academic achievement and was the strongest predictor of high school dropout risk. Previous work has revealed that kindergarten math competence is a key contributor to academic achievement in elementary school students.35  Although vocabulary skills were not related to academic outcomes beyond math, they did predict subsequent decreases in self-reported anxiety sensitivity in adolescence. Large-scale population-based findings have shown that kindergarten math skills become more important than vocabulary,3  suggesting that the influence of language becomes indirect, as children transition from learning to read to reading to learn, over the course of elementary and high school.33  With our findings, we also suggest that solid vocabulary skills promise an important edge for psychological adjustment and, ultimately, overall mental health.

Children’s kindergarten classroom engagement was a consistent predictor of high school outcomes predicting academic achievement, dropout risk, school connectedness, substance use, physical activity, and weight status. Previous researchers have found that being less invested in school is associated with underachievement, reduced academic motivation, less supportive relationships with teachers, more involvement with delinquent peers, and a higher frequency of problem behaviors including substance use.3436  Our results are consistent with this literature and suggest that the roots of less-effective engagement may be traceable to kindergarten.

Classroom engagement skills are also strongly reflective of impulse control, goal-directedness, and cognitive and attentional control.37  Children need self-control to follow classroom rules, work cooperatively with other students, and remain focused in the face of distractions. As such, it is possible that classroom engagement represents an important mechanism through which self-control ultimately contributes to academic adjustment.38  In line with this hypothesis, researchers have found that classroom behavior accounts for the association between executive functioning and achievement.39  Furthermore, other researchers have linked preschool self-control to better economic, health, and interpersonal functioning in adulthood.40 

Children’s ability to learn at the time of school entry reflects the quality of their experiences and relationships during the preschool years. Sensitive caregiving, linguistically and cognitively stimulating environments, and protection from chronic adversity and stress support optimal brain development and capacity for lifelong learning, wellbeing, and health.41  In contrast, there is evidence that growing up in poverty is associated with developmental risks for regional gray matter deficits in the frontal and temporal lobes and hippocampi, which are critical factors in supporting the development school readiness skills.42  Children who grow up in socioeconomically disadvantaged families are therefore especially likely to begin school less well prepared than their more-advantaged peers.43 

Several interventions can help offset the significant burden experienced by vulnerable children. These include access to high quality universal preschool44  and community support systems such as home visitation45  and parental training programs.46,47  In addition to providing young children with experiences that are cognitively challenging, protection from toxic stress and adversity also represent essential components of impactful interventions.41  Encouragingly, home visit programs have been found to have positive effects on child health and school readiness and offer mechanisms for intervening to reduce parent stress.

Pediatricians can play a key role in promoting school readiness by helping ensure that children receive the nutrition, sleep, and developmental experiences they need to develop strong school readiness and learning skills.48  In addition, pediatricians can screen children for exposure to environmental threats including lead, toxins, and excessive screen time, as well as exposure to chronic adversity, which can take the form of housing or food insecurity, family violence, parental mental health problems, and the experience of discrimination and systemic racism. Finally, when needed, pediatricians can share information on community resources with parents.

The present research is not without limitations. First, in our study, we did not account for preschool attendance. As a result, it is possible that this unmeasured variable accounts for some of the variance in school readiness. Given the tremendous popularity of affordable day care policy implementation in the province of Quebec, we estimate that preschool participation is unlikely to represent an important confounding variable. Furthermore, the contextual variable most closely related to preschool participation in Quebec is socioeconomic status, which we statistically adjust for in our analyses.49  Second, our correlational design precludes us from making any causal attributions. It may be the case that important unmeasured child and family characteristics are responsible for the observed associations. Nevertheless, to reduce the possibility of confounding, we accounted for influential child (nonverbal IQ) and family (socioeconomic status, parental involvement) characteristics in our analyses. Third, the current study was based on secondary data, and as a result, direct assessments for outcome measures were nonexistent. Consequently, it is possible that social desirability motives or recall bias led to some of misclassification on the behalf of respondents. Nevertheless, the use of multiple measures, including direct assessments as well as teacher and self-reports, allows us to reduce the chances of shared measurement bias. The use of prospective longitudinal data, which allowed us to assess child school readiness long before they begin to develop dispositions toward health and productivity and to study the evolution of a large population-based group of children over the course of 17 years, represents compelling strengths.

High school dropout incurs high medical and social costs. In particular, dropout is associated with increased risk of poverty, nicotine dependence, low self-esteem, depression, underemployment, and involvement in criminal behavior.5052  Given that high school dropout exacts a heavy cost on individuals and society, in the current study, we underscore that early interventions which aim to boost school readiness could translate into important economic savings across the life span. With our findings, we further highlight the usefulness of conceptualizing kindergarten readiness as a public health issue53,54  and indicate that the decreased quality of life experienced by youth who fail to complete high school may be rooted in early childhood risk.

The QLSCD was supported by funding from the Quebec Government’s Ministry of Health, Ministry of Education, Ministry of Family Affairs, the Lucie and Andre Chagnon Foundation, the Robert-Sauve Research Institute of Health and Security at Work, and the Quebec Statistics Institute. Additional funding was received by Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and Nova Scotia Research.

Dr Fitzpatrick had full access to the data, conceptualized the study, conducted the analyses, and drafted the manuscript; Drs Boers and Pagani provided critical feedback and edited the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

FUNDING: No external funding.

NKT

Number Knowledge Test

QLSCD

Quebec Longitudinal Study of Child Development

1
Williams
PG
,
Lerner
MA
;
Council on Early Childhood, Council on School Health
.
School readiness
.
Pediatrics
.
2019
;
144
(
2
):
e20191766
2
Davies
S
,
Janus
M
,
Duku
E
,
Gaskin
A
.
Using the Early Development Instrument to examine cognitive and non-cognitive school readiness and elementary student achievement
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2016
;
35
:
63
75
3
Duncan
GJ
,
Dowsett
CJ
,
Claessens
A
, et al
.
School readiness and later achievement
.
Dev Psychol
.
2007
;
43
(
6
):
1428
1446
4
Romano
E
,
Babchishin
L
,
Pagani
LS
,
Kohen
D
.
School readiness and later achievement: replication and extension using a nationwide Canadian survey
.
Dev Psychol
.
2010
;
46
(
5
):
995
1007
5
Pagani
LS
,
Fitzpatrick
C
,
Archambault
I
,
Janosz
M
.
School readiness and later achievement: a French Canadian replication and extension
.
Dev Psychol
.
2010
;
46
(
5
):
984
994
6
Fitzpatrick
C
,
Pagani
LS
.
Task-oriented kindergarten behavior pays off in later childhood
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2013
;
34
(
2
):
94
101
7
Entwisle
DR
,
Alexander
KL
,
Olson
LS
.
First grade and educational attainment by age 22: a new story
.
Am J Sociol
.
2005
;
110
:
1458
1502
8
Pagani
LS
,
Fitzpatrick
C
.
Children’s school readiness: implications for eliminating future disparities in health and education
.
Health Educ Behav
.
2014
;
41
(
1
):
25
33
9
Ladd
GW
,
Dinella
LM
.
Continuity and change in early school engagement: predictive of children’s achievement trajectories from first to eighth grade?
J Educ Psychol
.
2009
;
101
(
1
):
190
206
10
McWayne
C
,
Hampton
V
,
Fantuzzo
J
,
Cohen
HL
,
Sekino
Y
.
A multivariate examination of parent involvement and the social and academic competencies of urban kindergarten children
.
Psychol Sch
.
2004
;
41
:
363
377
11
Piché
G
,
Fitzpatrick
C
,
Pagani
LS
.
Associations between extracurricular activity and self-regulation: a longitudinal study from 5 to 10 years of age
.
Am J Health Promot
.
2015
;
30
(
1
):
e32
e40
12
Elovainio
M
,
Rosenström
T
,
Hakulinen
C
, et al
.
Educational attainment and health transitions over the life course: testing the potential mechanisms
.
J Public Health (Oxf)
.
2016
;
38
(
3
):
e254
e262
13
Cohen
AK
,
Syme
SL
.
Education: a missed opportunity for public health intervention
.
Am J Public Health
.
2013
;
103
(
6
):
997
1001
14
Woolf
SH
,
Braveman
P
.
Where health disparities begin: the role of social and economic determinants--and why current policies may make matters worse
.
Health Aff (Millwood)
.
2011
;
30
(
10
):
1852
1859
15
Cicchetti
D
, ed..
Developmental Psychopathology, Theory and Method
, 3rd ed.
Hoboken, NJ
:
John Wiley & Sons
;
2016
16
Sameroff
A
.
A unified theory of development: a dialectic integration of nature and nurture
.
Child Dev
.
2010
;
81
(
1
):
6
22
17
Okamoto
Y
,
Case
R
.
Exploring the microstructure of children’s central conceptual structures in the domain of number
.
Monogr Soc Res Child Dev
.
1996
;
61
(
1–2
):
27
58
18
Dunn
LM
,
Thériault-Whalen
CM
,
Dunn
LM
, eds..
Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised: French Adaptation
.
Toronto, Canada
:
Psycan
;
1993
19
Desrosiers
H
,
Ducharme
A
. Commencer L’école Du Bon Pied. Facteurs Associés À L’acquisition Du Vocabulaire À La Fin de La Maternelle.
Québec, Canada
:
Institut de la statistique Québec
;
2006
20
Archambault
I
,
Janosz
M
,
Fallu
JS
,
Pagani
LS
.
Student engagement and its relationship with early high school dropout
.
J Adolesc
.
2009
;
32
(
3
):
651
670
21
Woicik
PA
,
Stewart
SH
,
Pihl
RO
,
Conrod
PJ
.
The Substance Use Risk Profile Scale: a scale measuring traits linked to reinforcement-specific substance use profiles
.
Addict Behav
.
2009
;
34
(
12
):
1042
1055
22
Wechsler
D
.
WISC-III: Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: Manual
.
Psychological Corporation
;
1991
23
Achenbach
TM
,
Edelbrock
C
,
Howell
CT
.
Empirically based assessment of the behavioral/emotional problems of 2- and 3- year-old children
.
J Abnorm Child Psychol
.
1987
;
15
(
4
):
629
650
24
Behar
L
,
Stringfield
S
.
A behavior rating scale for the preschool child
.
Dev Psychol
.
1974
;
10
:
601
610
25
Fowler
PC
,
Park
RM
.
Factor structure of the preschool behavior questionnaire in a normal population
.
Psychol Rep
.
1979
;
45
:
599
606
26
Bradley
RH
,
Whiteside
L
,
Mundfrom
DJ
,
Casey
PH
,
Kelleher
KJ
,
Pope
SK
.
Early indications of resilience and their relation to experiences in the home environments of low birthweight, premature children living in poverty
.
Child Dev
.
1994
;
65
(
2, spec no
):
346
360
27
Willms
JD
,
Shields
M
.
A Measure of Socioeconomic Status for the National Longitudinal Study of Children
.
Fredericton, NB
:
Atlantic Center for Policy Research in Education, University of New Brunswick and Statistics Canada
;
1996
28
Li
P
,
Stuart
EA
,
Allison
DB
.
Multiple imputation: a flexible tool for handling missing data
.
JAMA
.
2015
;
314
(
18
):
1966
1967
29
Skinner
AC
,
Ravanbakht
SN
,
Skelton
JA
,
Perrin
EM
,
Armstrong
SC
.
Prevalence of obesity and severe obesity in US children, 1999–2016
.
Pediatrics
.
2018
;
141
(
3
):
e20173459
30
Mojtabai
R
,
Olfson
M
,
Han
B
.
National trends in the prevalence and treatment of depression in adolescents and young adults
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
6
):
e20161878
31
Curby
TW
,
Rimm-Kaufman
SE
,
Ponitz
CC
.
Teacher–child interactions and children’s achievement trajectories across kindergarten and first grade
.
J Ed Psych
.
2009
;
101
:
912
32
Penno
JF
,
Wilkinson
IA
,
Moore
DW
.
Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: do they overcome the Matthew effect?
J Ed Psych
.
2002
;
94
:
23
33
33
Pagani
LS
,
Brière
FN
,
Janosz
M
.
Fluid reasoning skills at the high school transition predict subsequent dropout
.
Intelligence
.
2017
;
62
:
48
53
34
Wang
MT
,
Fredricks
JA
.
The reciprocal links between school engagement, youth problem behaviors, and school dropout during adolescence
.
Child Dev
.
2014
;
85
(
2
):
722
737
35
Reeve
J
,
Lee
W
.
Students’ classroom engagement produces longitudinal changes in classroom motivation
.
J Ed Psych
.
2014
;
106
:
527
540
36
Skinner
EA
,
Pitzer
J
. Developmental Dynamics of Engagement, Coping, and Everyday Resilience. In:
Christenson
SA
,
Reschly
AL
,
Wylie
C
, eds.
The Handbook of Research on Student Engagement
.
New York
:
Springer Science
;
2012
:
21
44
37
Hagger
MS
,
Wood
C
,
Stiff
C
,
Chatzisarantis
NLD
.
Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis
.
Psychol Bull
.
2010
;
136
(
4
):
495
525
38
Rimm-Kaufman
SE
,
Curby
TW
,
Grimm
KJ
,
Nathanson
L
,
Brock
LL
.
The contribution of children’s self-regulation and classroom quality to children’s adaptive behaviors in the kindergarten classroom
.
Dev Psychol
.
2009
;
45
(
4
):
958
972
39
Sasser
TR
,
Bierman
KL
,
Heinrichs
B
.
Executive functioning and school adjustment: the mediational role of pre-kindergarten learning-related behaviors
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2015
;
30
(
pt A
):
70
79
40
Moffitt
TE
,
Arseneault
L
,
Belsky
D
, et al
.
A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2011
;
108
(
7
):
2693
2698
41
Shonkoff
JP
.
Capitalizing on advances in science to reduce the health consequences of early childhood adversity
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2016
;
170
(
10
):
1003
1007
42
Hair
NL
,
Hanson
JL
,
Wolfe
BL
,
Pollak
SD
.
Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2015
;
169
(
9
):
822
829
43
Isaacs
JB
.
Starting School at a Disadvantage: The School Readiness of Poor Children
.
Washington, DC
:
Center on Children and Families at Brookings
;
2012
44
van Huizen
T
,
Dumhs
L
,
Plantenga
J
.
The costs and benefits of investing in universal preschool: evidence from a Spanish reform
.
Child Dev
.
2019
;
90
(
3
):
e386
e406
45
Duffee
JH
,
Mendelsohn
AL
,
Kuo
AA
,
Legano
LA
,
Earls
MF
;
Council on Community Pediatrics, Council on Early Childhood, Committee on Child Abuse and Neglect
.
Early childhood home visiting
.
Pediatrics
.
2017
;
140
(
3
):
e20172150
46
Landry
SH
,
Zucker
TA
,
Williams
JM
,
Merz
EC
,
Guttentag
CL
,
Taylor
HB
.
Improving school readiness of high-risk preschoolers: combining high quality instructional strategies with responsive training for teachers and parents
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2017
;
40
:
38
51
47
Nix
RL
,
Bierman
KL
,
Motamedi
M
,
Heinrichs
BS
,
Gill
S
.
Parent engagement in a Head Start home visiting program predicts sustained growth in children’s school readiness
.
Early Child Res Q
.
2018
;
45
:
106
114
48
Council on Early Childhood, Council on School Health
.
The pediatrician’s role in optimizing school readiness
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
3
):
e20162293
49
Geoffroy
MC
,
Séguin
JR
,
Lacourse
E
,
Boivin
M
,
Tremblay
RE
,
Côté
SM
.
Parental characteristics associated with childcare use during the first 4 years of life: results from a representative cohort of Québec families
.
Can J Public Health
.
2012
;
103
(
1
):
76
80
50
McCaul
EJ
,
Donaldson
GA
 Jr.
,
Coladarci
T
,
Davis
WE
.
Consequences of dropping out of school: findings from high school and beyond
.
J Educ Res
.
1992
;
85
:
198
207
51
Campbell
C
.
The socioeconomic consequences of dropping out of high school: evidence from an analysis of siblings
.
Soc Sci Res
.
2015
;
51
:
108
118
52
Maynard
BR
,
Salas-Wright
CP
,
Vaughn
MG
.
High school dropouts in emerging adulthood: substance use, mental health problems, and crime
.
Community Ment Health J
.
2015
;
51
(
3
):
289
299
53
Freudenberg
N
,
Ruglis
J
.
Reframing school dropout as a public health issue
.
Prev Chronic Dis
.
2007
;
4
(
4
):
A107
54
Woolf
SH
,
Aron
LY
.
The US health disadvantage relative to other high-income countries: findings from a National Research Council/Institute of Medicine report
.
JAMA
.
2013
;
309
(
8
):
771
772

Competing Interests

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Supplementary data