Video Abstract

Video Abstract

Close modal
BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES:

The Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC), one of the largest US safety net programs, was revised in 2009 to be more congruent with dietary guidelines. We hypothesize that this revision led to improvements in child development.

METHODS:

Data were drawn from a cohort of women and children enrolled in the Conditions Affecting Neurocognitive Development and Learning in Early Childhood study from 2006 to 2011 (Shelby County, TN; N = 1222). Using quasi-experimental difference-in-differences analysis, we compared measures of growth, cognitive, and socioemotional development between WIC recipients and nonrecipients before and after the policy revision.

RESULTS:

The revised WIC food package led to increased length-for-age z scores at 12 months among infants whose mothers received the revised food package during pregnancy (β = .33, 95% confidence interval: 0.05 to 0.61) and improved Bayley Scales of Infant Development cognitive composite scores at 24 months (β = 4.34, 95% confidence interval: 1.11 to 7.57). We observed no effects on growth at age 24 months or age 4 to 6 years or cognitive development at age 4 to 6 years.

CONCLUSIONS:

This study provides some of the first evidence that children of mothers who received the revised WIC food package during pregnancy had improved developmental outcomes in the first 2 years of life. These findings highlight the value of WIC in improving early developmental outcomes among vulnerable children. The need to implement and expand policies supporting the health of marginalized groups has never been more salient, particularly given the nation’s rising economic and social disparities.

What’s Known on This Subject:

Researchers in previous studies have demonstrated that revisions to the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children in October 2009 led to improved healthy food access, dietary quality, and birth outcomes among low-income families.

What This Study Adds:

Using unique cohort data and a quasi-experimental design, we found that the 2009 revisions to the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children also improved early childhood developmental outcomes, suggesting that nutritional interventions during pregnancy may reduce intergenerational transmission of health disparities.

Maternal nutrition plays a critical role in fetal growth and development.15  Suboptimal maternal nutrition during pregnancy may shape the programming of fetal tissues, which is thought to increase chronic disease risk in childhood and adulthood.69  To ensure optimal birth outcomes, child growth, and brain development, the nutritional demands of fetal development need to be met. In the United States, the unequal distribution of health care, social support, and access to healthy food has led to racial and socioeconomic disparities in maternal nutrition.1012  Evidence suggests that nutritional interventions during pregnancy could substantially improve birth outcomes and subsequent child health and development, including cognitive development and physical growth.1317  As such, targeted policies and programs that improve access to healthy foods in at-risk communities could be a promising approach for addressing social disparities in child outcomes that have been associated with maternal nutrition during pregnancy.16,17 

Established in the 1970s, the US Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) was created to safeguard the health of women, infants, and children up to the age of 5 living in low-income households18  in part through the provision of healthy foods. WIC serves approximately half of all infants, 30% of all children aged <5, and approximately one-third of all pregnant and postpartum women.19  In 2019, >$5 billion was spent to aid ∼6.4 million women, infants, and children enrolled in WIC nationwide.20  WIC serves marginalized communities, with ∼50% of children being Hispanic or Latino and 20% being African American.21  In 2009, the WIC food packages for both women and children were revised for the first time to better align it with national dietary guidelines.22,23  This revision included healthier food options (eg, fruits and vegetables), restrictions to less-healthy options (eg, requiring milk to be low-fat and bread to be whole grain), and incentives for breastfeeding.19  Importantly, the revised WIC food package introduced $10-per-month vouchers for purchase of fresh fruits and vegetables.

Extant literature has found that the revised WIC food package improved healthy food availability,2427  maternal and child dietary quality,2831  perinatal and birth outcomes,32  and breastfeeding.3335  However, no researchers, to our knowledge, have evaluated effects of this revision on downstream child health or development. In this study, we address this gap in our understanding of one of the largest US safety net programs by examining the association between the revised food package and several early childhood developmental outcomes. By employing a quasi-experimental difference-in-differences (DiD) design, we estimated effects of the revised food package on child growth, socioemotional, and neurodevelopmental outcomes. We hypothesized that children whose mothers received the revised WIC food package during pregnancy would have better developmental outcomes.

The sample consisted of mothers and their children from the Conditions Affecting Neurocognitive Development and Learning in Early Childhood (CANDLE) longitudinal cohort study, which enrolled 1503 pregnant women from Memphis and surrounding Shelby County, Tennessee during 2006–2011.36  Longitudinal data collection occurred during pregnancy and throughout childhood. We included data from the baseline survey during pregnancy to capture key covariates and later survey waves that included the outcomes of interest. The sample was restricted to children for whom valid data were collected on these outcomes (Fig 1).

FIGURE 1

Sample flowchart.

FIGURE 1

Sample flowchart.

Close modal

Exposure

The main exposure was whether women received the revised WIC food package during pregnancy. Because the revision was implemented in October 2009, women who gave birth after this date were considered to be exposed during their pregnancy.37  Participation in WIC during pregnancy was self-reported retrospectively during postpartum survey waves. Unfortunately, CANDLE did not ask women about their receipt of WIC during the postpartum period, during which they would have also been eligible for breastfeeding assistance, so in this study we focused on WIC receipt during pregnancy as the primary exposure.

Outcomes

We considered 3 core child developmental outcome domains measured at 1, 2, and 4 to 6 years of age, each of which has been previously linked with maternal nutrition: child growth, cognitive development, and socioemotional development. Below, we describe in detail those outcomes that fulfilled the assumptions of our statistical methods and were therefore included in our analyses (see Supplemental Information for more details on outcomes not included).

Child Growth, calculated by using Centers for Disease Control and Prevention growth charts,38  was assessed by using length-for-age z scores at 12 months, weight-for-age and weight-for-length z scores at 24 months, and height-for-age z scores at 4 to 6 years. Researchers in several studies have documented potential long-term consequences of failure to thrive in infancy, including adverse cognitive outcomes.39,40  Early childhood growth has also been linked to subsequent growth and height in adulthood,41,42  which has been found to be correlated with improved adult outcomes even in high-income countries.43,44  Other anthropometric measures (eg, length for age at 24 months) were not included because they did not meet the assumptions of statistical models (see Supplemental Information).

Socioemotional development was measured with the Brief Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment (BITSEA).45  Assessments were conducted by a licensed psychologist or advanced graduate student in psychology at both 12 and 24 months of age; we include each of these time points as a separate outcome. The BITSEA consists of 2 multiitem scales: the competence score for socioemotional abilities and the problem total scale for behavioral problems and emotional dysregulation. Socioemotional development at age 4 to 6 years, measured by the Child Behavior Checklist, was also assessed but not included because of violations of the statistical model assumptions (see Supplemental Information).

Cognitive development was assessed at age 24 months by a licensed psychologist using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development (Bayley).46  The Bayley at 24 months is correlated with full-scale IQ and school readiness during preschool.47  Correlations are especially strong among children at developmental risk.48,49  We included 3 Bayley scores: the cognitive composite score, receptive communication subtest scaled score, and total language subtest composite score. We also included a standardized full-scale child IQ score, assessed at 4 to 6 years of age by trained psychologists using the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales (SB-5).50  The SB-5 has been validated in large, diverse populations and serves as a reliable assessment of intellectual and cognitive abilities. The Bayley expressive communication subtest was also assessed but not included because of violations to the statistical models (see Supplemental Information).

Covariates

Models included covariates that might confound the relationship between receipt of the revised WIC food package and the outcomes: mother’s age, educational attainment, marital status, race, receipt of Medicaid during pregnancy, household size, household income, and total number of pregnancies and child’s sex. We included indicator variables for birthyear in all models to account for secular trends.

First, we calculated descriptive statistics stratified by WIC receipt during pregnancy. Then, we estimated the effects of the revised food package using DiD analysis. DiD analysis is a quasi-experimental technique, ideally suited to examine the effects of policy changes while accounting for secular trends.51  We leveraged the fact that revisions to the WIC food package in October 2009 were unlikely to be confounded by participant characteristics. Thus, we compared outcomes before and after the policy revision among WIC recipients, “differencing out” secular trends in outcomes among nonrecipients, which included both noneligible individuals and eligible nonrecipients. We chose this as our primary comparison group rather than WIC-eligible participants because CANDLE collected income as a crude categorical rather than a continuous variable; this would have introduced significant measurement error in identifying WIC-eligible families. We included a comparison with families likely to be WIC eligible as a sensitivity analysis (see below). Additional details on the DiD model, including the equation and testing of standard assumptions, are included in the Supplemental Information.

We conducted several analyses to test the robustness of the results. First, we used WIC eligibility as the exposure, rather than WIC receipt (n = 967, n = 937, and n = 879 for outcomes measured at age 12 months, 24 months, and 4–6 years, respectively). WIC eligibility is defined by income standards up to 185% of the federal poverty threshold, enrollment in other social assistance programs, and nutrition risk. Roughly 50% of pregnant women in the United States who are eligible actually receive WIC benefits,52  so this analysis is analogous to an intent-to-treat approach. This analysis allowed us to correct for potential bias from “nonadherence,” because eligible women who enroll in WIC could differ from eligible women who do not enroll. By using CANDLE’s categorical income data, eligibility was computed by using WIC eligibility guidelines for income, household size, and Medicaid receipt. Second, we restricted the sample to women whose pregnancies did not include the WIC revision date (October 2009) to examine associations among women exposed to the revised WIC package throughout their entire pregnancy (n = 749, n = 727, n = 668 for outcomes measured at age 12 months, 24 months, and 4–6 years, respectively). Third, we restricted our analysis to women who made <$75 000 per year to be more reflective of women eligible for WIC (n = 777, n = 758, and n = 701 for outcomes measured at age 12 months, 24 months, and 4–6 years, respectively). Fourth, visual inspection of the parallel trends plots indicated that trends in outcomes between WIC recipients and nonrecipients were most comparable in the 20 months before the WIC revision. Thus, we restricted the sample to observations occurring within 20 months of the WIC revision (n = 918, n = 894, n = 818 for outcomes measured at age 12 months, 24 months, and 4–6 years, respectively).

More than half (56.9%) those who received WIC during pregnancy gave birth after October 2009 (Table 1). Overall, 87.2% of WIC recipients and 33.1% of nonrecipients were Black. Additionally, 24.2% of WIC recipients had more than a high school education, compared with 70.8% of nonrecipients. WIC recipients were more likely to be younger (24.6, SD: 5.1) compared with nonrecipients (29.0, SD: 4.9).

TABLE 1

Sample Characteristics

CharacteristicNo WIC in Pregnancy, n = 525WIC in Pregnancy, n = 700
Mean (SD)%Mean (SD)%
Gave birth after October 2009 — 43.1 — 56.9 
Sociodemographic     
 Mother’s age 29.0 (4.9)  24.6 (5.1)  
 Mother’s marital status — 86.3 — 37.1 
 Annual household income     
  $0–$9999 — 6.3 — 31.7 
  $10 000–$24 999 — 7.3 — 32.3 
  $25 000–$44 999 — 15.3 — 21.0 
  $45 000–$74 999 — 33.3 — 12.7 
  $75 000+ — 37.8 — 2.0 
 Female child — 51.0 — 49.6 
 Mother’s race     
  Black — 33.1 — 87.2 
  White or othera — 66.9 — 12.9 
 Mother’s education     
  Less than high school — 4.3 — 14.0 
  High school — 25.0 — 61.8 
  More than high school — 70.8 — 24.2 
 Medicaid or TennCare — 21.6 — 82.2 
 Total No. pregnancies 2.4 (1.4) — 2.7 (1.8) — 
 No. people in household 4.0 (1.2) — 4.6 (1.6) — 
Child health outcomes at age 12 mo     
 Length-for-age z score 0.05 (1.2) — 0.04 (1.2) — 
 BITSEA: problem total 8.0 (4.9) — 10.9 (6.1) — 
Child health outcomes at age 24 mo     
 Wt-for-age z score 2.1 (1.1) — 2.0 (1.1) — 
 Wt-for-length z score 0.4 (1.2) — 0.4 (1.2) — 
 BITSEA: problem total 8.3 (5.5) — 11.3 (6.7) — 
 BITSEA: competency total 18.5 (2.4) — 17.7 (2.7) — 
 Bayley: cognitive composite score 102.5 (14.1) — 94.4 (10.9) — 
 Bayley: receptive communication subtest 10.5 (3.1) — 8.8 (2.4) — 
 Bayley: total language subtest 103.3 (17.0) — 94.8 (12.6) — 
Child health outcome at 4–6 y of age     
 Height-for-age z score 0.41 (1.0) — 0.48 (1.3) — 
 Cognitive test score 106.3 (14.7) — 96.2 (13.7) — 
CharacteristicNo WIC in Pregnancy, n = 525WIC in Pregnancy, n = 700
Mean (SD)%Mean (SD)%
Gave birth after October 2009 — 43.1 — 56.9 
Sociodemographic     
 Mother’s age 29.0 (4.9)  24.6 (5.1)  
 Mother’s marital status — 86.3 — 37.1 
 Annual household income     
  $0–$9999 — 6.3 — 31.7 
  $10 000–$24 999 — 7.3 — 32.3 
  $25 000–$44 999 — 15.3 — 21.0 
  $45 000–$74 999 — 33.3 — 12.7 
  $75 000+ — 37.8 — 2.0 
 Female child — 51.0 — 49.6 
 Mother’s race     
  Black — 33.1 — 87.2 
  White or othera — 66.9 — 12.9 
 Mother’s education     
  Less than high school — 4.3 — 14.0 
  High school — 25.0 — 61.8 
  More than high school — 70.8 — 24.2 
 Medicaid or TennCare — 21.6 — 82.2 
 Total No. pregnancies 2.4 (1.4) — 2.7 (1.8) — 
 No. people in household 4.0 (1.2) — 4.6 (1.6) — 
Child health outcomes at age 12 mo     
 Length-for-age z score 0.05 (1.2) — 0.04 (1.2) — 
 BITSEA: problem total 8.0 (4.9) — 10.9 (6.1) — 
Child health outcomes at age 24 mo     
 Wt-for-age z score 2.1 (1.1) — 2.0 (1.1) — 
 Wt-for-length z score 0.4 (1.2) — 0.4 (1.2) — 
 BITSEA: problem total 8.3 (5.5) — 11.3 (6.7) — 
 BITSEA: competency total 18.5 (2.4) — 17.7 (2.7) — 
 Bayley: cognitive composite score 102.5 (14.1) — 94.4 (10.9) — 
 Bayley: receptive communication subtest 10.5 (3.1) — 8.8 (2.4) — 
 Bayley: total language subtest 103.3 (17.0) — 94.8 (12.6) — 
Child health outcome at 4–6 y of age     
 Height-for-age z score 0.41 (1.0) — 0.48 (1.3) — 
 Cognitive test score 106.3 (14.7) — 96.2 (13.7) — 

N = 1287 (data on WIC receipt were missing for 62 participants). Sample includes children enrolled in the CANDLE Study, Shelby County, Tennessee, 2006–2011. —, not applicable.

a

Less than 2% of the sample were Hispanic or other race or ethnicity. Thus, they were included in the “white” category to avoid unstable estimates due to small cell sizes.

Compared with infants whose mothers did not receive the revised WIC food package during pregnancy, those whose mothers received the revised WIC food package had greater length-for-age z scores at age 12 months (β = .33, 95% confidence interval [CI]: −0.05 to 0.61) and higher Bayley cognitive composite score at age 24 months (β = 4.34, 95% CI:1.11 to 7.57) (Table 2). We observed no statistically significant association between receipt of the revised WIC package with child growth at 24 months or 4 to 6 years of age, measures of socioemotional development at both 12 and 24 months, or measures of cognitive development at 4 to 6 years of age.

TABLE 2

Effects of the Revised WIC Food Package on Child Health Outcomes

Outcomes at Age 12 moOutcomes at Age 24 moOutcomes at Age 4–6 y
Length-for-Age z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalWt-for-Age z ScoreWt-for-Length z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalBITSEA: Competency TotalBayley: Cognitive Composite ScoreBayley: Receptive Communication SubtestBayley: Total Language SubtestHeight-for-Age z ScoreCognitive Test Score
WIC × Postrevision 0.33** (0.05 to 0.61) −0.46 (−1.85 to 0.94) 0.20 (−0.10 to 0.51) 0.18 (−0.16 to 0.52) −0.49 (−2.11 to 1.12) −0.12 (−0.80 to 0.57) 4.34*** (1.11 to 7.57) −0.21 (−0.91 to 0.49) −0.39 (−4.13 to 3.35) 0.10 (−0.20 to 0.39) −0.07 (−3.59 to 3.45) 
WIC −0.18 (−0.44 to 0.08) 0.29 (−1.02 to 1.60) −0.15 (−0.44 to 0.14) −0.09 (−0.41 to 0.23) 0.61 (−0.93 to 2.15) −0.04 (−0.69 to 0.61) −3.53**(−6.60 to −0.46) 0.26 (−0.40 to 0.92) 0.84 (−2.71 to 4.40) 0.03 (−0.25 to 0.31) 0.43 (−2.86 to 3.71) 
Postrevision 0.16 (−0.16 to 0.48) 0.26 (−1.34 to 1.85) 0.23 (−0.10 to 0.57) 0.37* (−0.00 to 0.75) −0.82 (−2.62 to 0.99) −0.02 (−0.79 to 0.74) −2.54 (−6.14 to 1.06) 0.06 (−0.71 to 0.84) 0.37 (−3.80 to 4.54) 0.11 (−0.24 to 0.45) 0.09 (−3.92 to 4.09) 
Outcomes at Age 12 moOutcomes at Age 24 moOutcomes at Age 4–6 y
Length-for-Age z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalWt-for-Age z ScoreWt-for-Length z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalBITSEA: Competency TotalBayley: Cognitive Composite ScoreBayley: Receptive Communication SubtestBayley: Total Language SubtestHeight-for-Age z ScoreCognitive Test Score
WIC × Postrevision 0.33** (0.05 to 0.61) −0.46 (−1.85 to 0.94) 0.20 (−0.10 to 0.51) 0.18 (−0.16 to 0.52) −0.49 (−2.11 to 1.12) −0.12 (−0.80 to 0.57) 4.34*** (1.11 to 7.57) −0.21 (−0.91 to 0.49) −0.39 (−4.13 to 3.35) 0.10 (−0.20 to 0.39) −0.07 (−3.59 to 3.45) 
WIC −0.18 (−0.44 to 0.08) 0.29 (−1.02 to 1.60) −0.15 (−0.44 to 0.14) −0.09 (−0.41 to 0.23) 0.61 (−0.93 to 2.15) −0.04 (−0.69 to 0.61) −3.53**(−6.60 to −0.46) 0.26 (−0.40 to 0.92) 0.84 (−2.71 to 4.40) 0.03 (−0.25 to 0.31) 0.43 (−2.86 to 3.71) 
Postrevision 0.16 (−0.16 to 0.48) 0.26 (−1.34 to 1.85) 0.23 (−0.10 to 0.57) 0.37* (−0.00 to 0.75) −0.82 (−2.62 to 0.99) −0.02 (−0.79 to 0.74) −2.54 (−6.14 to 1.06) 0.06 (−0.71 to 0.84) 0.37 (−3.80 to 4.54) 0.11 (−0.24 to 0.45) 0.09 (−3.92 to 4.09) 

Sample includes children enrolled in the CANDLE Study, Shelby County, Tennessee, 2006–2011. n = 956 for outcomes assessed at age 12-months, n = 929 for outcomes assessed at age 24-months, n = 844 for outcome assessed at 4–6 years old. The above estimates represent β coefficients with 95% CIs in parentheses. The effect estimate of interest is the coefficient on the interaction term. Analyses involved regressing each of the outcomes listed above on an indicator for WIC receipt, an indicator for whether the birth occurred after October 2009, and the interaction between the two. The models additionally adjusted for mother’s age, educational attainment, marital status, race, receipt of Medicaid during pregnancy, household size, household income, and total number of pregnancies and child’s sex.

***

P < .01; ** P < .05; * P < .10

When we considered WIC-eligible individuals as the treatment group (rather than recipients) (Table 3, model 1), and when we excluded pregnancies including October 2009 (model 2), results for length for age remained similar to the primary analysis. However, we noted an attenuated effect size and a CI that included the null for the Bayley cognitive composite score. When we excluded women with incomes of $75 000 or more (model 3), the association with the cognitive score remained similar to the primary analysis. However, we noted a decreased effect size and CI that included the null for length for age. When we excluded observations occurring >20 months before the revision, the results remained similar to the primary analysis (model 4).

TABLE 3

Sensitivity Analyses: Effect of the Revised WIC Food Packages on Child Health Outcomes

Outcomes at Age 12 moOutcomes at Age 24 moOutcomes at Age 4–6 y
Length-for-Age z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalWt-for-Age z ScoreWt-for-Length z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalBITSEA: Competency TotalBayley: Cognitive Composite ScoreBayley: Receptive Communication SubtestBayley: Total Language SubtestHeight-for-Age z ScoreCognitive Test Score
Model 1: WIC eligible            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.39*** (0.10 to 0.67) −0.08 (−1.51 to 1.35) 0.29* (−0.02 to 0.60) 0.12 (−0.23 to 0.46) −0.68 (−2.33 to 0.97) −0.03 (−0.73 to 0.66) 1.87 (−1.43 to 5.17) −0.28 (−0.99 to 0.43) −0.33 (−4.13 to 3.47) 0.13 (−0.18 to 0.43) −1.75 (−5.30 to 1.81) 
 WIC −0.03 (−0.57 to 0.51) 0.85 (−1.84 to 3.53) 0.04 (−0.53 to 0.62) 0.16 (−0.49 to 0.81) 2.07 (−1.03 to 5.17) 0.46 (−0.86 to 1.77) 3.68 (−2.52 to 9.88) 0.89 (−0.45 to 2.22) 4.97 (−2.17 to 12.11) −0.04 (−0.59 to 0.52) 5.16 (−1.35 to 11.67) 
 Postrevision 0.15 (−0.16 to 0.46) 0.07 (−1.50 to 1.64) 0.21 (−0.12 to 0.54) 0.41** (0.04 to 0.78) −0.73 (−2.50 to 1.04) −0.07 (−0.82 to 0.68) −1.27 (−4.81 to 2.28) 0.10 (−0.67 to 0.86) 0.39 (−3.68 to 4.47) 0.10 (−0.25 to 0.44) 0.87 (−3.08 to 4.82) 
 Observations 967 967 866 912 937 937 937 937 937 879 865 
Model 2: exclude October 2009 pregnancies            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.30* (−0.01 to 0.61) −0.02 (−1.54 to 1.49) 0.23 (−0.09 to 0.56) 0.13 (−0.25 to 0.51) −0.41 (−2.12 to 1.31) −0.17 (−0.92 to 0.59) 3.64** (0.07 to 7.20) −0.49 (−1.26 to 0.28) −1.42 (−5.56 to 2.71) 0.15 (−0.18 to 0.48) 0.52 (−3.41 to 4.44) 
 WIC −0.21 (−0.49 to 0.07) 0.23 (−1.14 to 1.59) −0.18 (−0.48 to 0.12) −0.10 (−0.44 to 0.25) 0.38 (−1.17 to 1.93) 0.13 (−0.55 to 0.81) −3.52** (−6.74 to −0.29) 0.39 (−0.31 to 1.08) 1.56 (−2.18 to 5.30) −0.03 (−0.32 to 0.27) 0.32 (−3.11 to 3.76) 
 Postrevision −0.01 (−0.50 to 0.47) −1.96 (−4.31 to 0.39) −0.04 (−0.57 to 0.50) 0.42 (−0.19 to 1.02) −3.79*** (−6.49 to −1.10) 0.68 (−0.52 to 1.87) −2.30 (−7.92 to 3.31) 1.69*** (0.48 to 2.91) 6.89** (0.38 to 13.41) 0.03 (−0.47 to 0.52) 4.91* (−0.93 to 10.75) 
 Observations 746 747 666 703 726 726 726 726 726 668 654 
Model 3: exclude income ≥$75 000            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.26 (−0.06 to 0.57) −0.18 (−1.87 to 1.50) 0.18 (−0.18 to 0.53) 0.15 (−0.24 to 0.54) −0.65 (−2.63 to 1.33) 0.18 (−0.63 to 0.99) 4.72** (1.03 to 8.41) −0.27 (−1.04 to 0.50) −0.20 (−4.33 to 3.93) 0.01 (−0.34 to 0.35) −0.46 (−4.56 to 3.64) 
 WIC −0.09 (−0.37 to 0.19) 0.06 (−1.43 to 1.55) −0.10 (−0.41 to 0.22) −0.01 (−0.36 to 0.34) 0.72 (−1.04 to 2.49) −0.25 (−0.97 to 0.48) −4.09** (−7.39 to −0.79) 0.27 (−0.42 to 0.95) 0.29 (−3.40 to 3.97) 0.04 (−0.27 to 0.34) 0.83 (−2.73 to 4.40) 
 Postrevision 0.14 (−0.24 to 0.51) −0.10 (−2.10 to 1.91) 0.28 (−0.13 to 0.69) 0.50** (0.05 to 0.96) −0.92 (−3.23 to 1.39) −0.37 (−1.32 to 0.58) −1.62 (−5.93 to 2.69) 0.26 (−0.64 to 1.15) 0.52 (−4.30 to 5.35) 0.13 (−0.29 to 0.54) 1.13 (−3.73 to 5.98) 
 Observations 775 774 694 733 755 755 755 755 755 701 690 
Model 4: exclude observations >20 mo prerevision            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.39*** (0.10 to 0.67) −0.41 (−1.81 to 1.00) 0.25 (−0.07 to 0.56) 0.22 (−0.12 to 0.56) −0.77 (−2.40 to 0.86) −0.03 (−0.72 to 0.67) 4.46*** (1.13 to 7.80) −0.21 (−0.93 to 0.51) −0.63 (−4.46 to 3.20) 0.15 (−0.16 to 0.45) −0.58 (−4.14 to 2.97) 
 WIC −0.23 (−0.50 to 0.05) 0.21 (−1.13 to 1.54) −0.18 (−0.48 to 0.11) −0.13 (−0.46 to 0.20) 0.95 (−0.61 to 2.51) −0.16 (−0.83 to 0.50) −3.70** (−6.89 to −0.51) 0.26 (−0.43 to 0.94) 0.99 (−2.67 to 4.65) −0.00 (−0.29 to 0.29) 0.68 (−2.67 to 4.03) 
 Postrevision 0.13 (−0.19 to 0.45) 0.25 (−1.32 to 1.82) 0.22 (−0.12 to 0.55) 0.36* (−0.02 to 0.73) −0.67 (−2.46 to 1.12) −0.07 (−0.83 to 0.70) −2.57 (−6.23 to 1.08) 0.06 (−0.72 to 0.85) 0.45 (−3.75 to 4.65) 0.08 (−0.26 to 0.43) 0.37 (−3.61 to 4.35) 
 Observations 916 916 829 868 892 892 892 892 892 818 805 
Outcomes at Age 12 moOutcomes at Age 24 moOutcomes at Age 4–6 y
Length-for-Age z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalWt-for-Age z ScoreWt-for-Length z ScoreBITSEA: Problem TotalBITSEA: Competency TotalBayley: Cognitive Composite ScoreBayley: Receptive Communication SubtestBayley: Total Language SubtestHeight-for-Age z ScoreCognitive Test Score
Model 1: WIC eligible            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.39*** (0.10 to 0.67) −0.08 (−1.51 to 1.35) 0.29* (−0.02 to 0.60) 0.12 (−0.23 to 0.46) −0.68 (−2.33 to 0.97) −0.03 (−0.73 to 0.66) 1.87 (−1.43 to 5.17) −0.28 (−0.99 to 0.43) −0.33 (−4.13 to 3.47) 0.13 (−0.18 to 0.43) −1.75 (−5.30 to 1.81) 
 WIC −0.03 (−0.57 to 0.51) 0.85 (−1.84 to 3.53) 0.04 (−0.53 to 0.62) 0.16 (−0.49 to 0.81) 2.07 (−1.03 to 5.17) 0.46 (−0.86 to 1.77) 3.68 (−2.52 to 9.88) 0.89 (−0.45 to 2.22) 4.97 (−2.17 to 12.11) −0.04 (−0.59 to 0.52) 5.16 (−1.35 to 11.67) 
 Postrevision 0.15 (−0.16 to 0.46) 0.07 (−1.50 to 1.64) 0.21 (−0.12 to 0.54) 0.41** (0.04 to 0.78) −0.73 (−2.50 to 1.04) −0.07 (−0.82 to 0.68) −1.27 (−4.81 to 2.28) 0.10 (−0.67 to 0.86) 0.39 (−3.68 to 4.47) 0.10 (−0.25 to 0.44) 0.87 (−3.08 to 4.82) 
 Observations 967 967 866 912 937 937 937 937 937 879 865 
Model 2: exclude October 2009 pregnancies            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.30* (−0.01 to 0.61) −0.02 (−1.54 to 1.49) 0.23 (−0.09 to 0.56) 0.13 (−0.25 to 0.51) −0.41 (−2.12 to 1.31) −0.17 (−0.92 to 0.59) 3.64** (0.07 to 7.20) −0.49 (−1.26 to 0.28) −1.42 (−5.56 to 2.71) 0.15 (−0.18 to 0.48) 0.52 (−3.41 to 4.44) 
 WIC −0.21 (−0.49 to 0.07) 0.23 (−1.14 to 1.59) −0.18 (−0.48 to 0.12) −0.10 (−0.44 to 0.25) 0.38 (−1.17 to 1.93) 0.13 (−0.55 to 0.81) −3.52** (−6.74 to −0.29) 0.39 (−0.31 to 1.08) 1.56 (−2.18 to 5.30) −0.03 (−0.32 to 0.27) 0.32 (−3.11 to 3.76) 
 Postrevision −0.01 (−0.50 to 0.47) −1.96 (−4.31 to 0.39) −0.04 (−0.57 to 0.50) 0.42 (−0.19 to 1.02) −3.79*** (−6.49 to −1.10) 0.68 (−0.52 to 1.87) −2.30 (−7.92 to 3.31) 1.69*** (0.48 to 2.91) 6.89** (0.38 to 13.41) 0.03 (−0.47 to 0.52) 4.91* (−0.93 to 10.75) 
 Observations 746 747 666 703 726 726 726 726 726 668 654 
Model 3: exclude income ≥$75 000            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.26 (−0.06 to 0.57) −0.18 (−1.87 to 1.50) 0.18 (−0.18 to 0.53) 0.15 (−0.24 to 0.54) −0.65 (−2.63 to 1.33) 0.18 (−0.63 to 0.99) 4.72** (1.03 to 8.41) −0.27 (−1.04 to 0.50) −0.20 (−4.33 to 3.93) 0.01 (−0.34 to 0.35) −0.46 (−4.56 to 3.64) 
 WIC −0.09 (−0.37 to 0.19) 0.06 (−1.43 to 1.55) −0.10 (−0.41 to 0.22) −0.01 (−0.36 to 0.34) 0.72 (−1.04 to 2.49) −0.25 (−0.97 to 0.48) −4.09** (−7.39 to −0.79) 0.27 (−0.42 to 0.95) 0.29 (−3.40 to 3.97) 0.04 (−0.27 to 0.34) 0.83 (−2.73 to 4.40) 
 Postrevision 0.14 (−0.24 to 0.51) −0.10 (−2.10 to 1.91) 0.28 (−0.13 to 0.69) 0.50** (0.05 to 0.96) −0.92 (−3.23 to 1.39) −0.37 (−1.32 to 0.58) −1.62 (−5.93 to 2.69) 0.26 (−0.64 to 1.15) 0.52 (−4.30 to 5.35) 0.13 (−0.29 to 0.54) 1.13 (−3.73 to 5.98) 
 Observations 775 774 694 733 755 755 755 755 755 701 690 
Model 4: exclude observations >20 mo prerevision            
 WIC × Postrevision 0.39*** (0.10 to 0.67) −0.41 (−1.81 to 1.00) 0.25 (−0.07 to 0.56) 0.22 (−0.12 to 0.56) −0.77 (−2.40 to 0.86) −0.03 (−0.72 to 0.67) 4.46*** (1.13 to 7.80) −0.21 (−0.93 to 0.51) −0.63 (−4.46 to 3.20) 0.15 (−0.16 to 0.45) −0.58 (−4.14 to 2.97) 
 WIC −0.23 (−0.50 to 0.05) 0.21 (−1.13 to 1.54) −0.18 (−0.48 to 0.11) −0.13 (−0.46 to 0.20) 0.95 (−0.61 to 2.51) −0.16 (−0.83 to 0.50) −3.70** (−6.89 to −0.51) 0.26 (−0.43 to 0.94) 0.99 (−2.67 to 4.65) −0.00 (−0.29 to 0.29) 0.68 (−2.67 to 4.03) 
 Postrevision 0.13 (−0.19 to 0.45) 0.25 (−1.32 to 1.82) 0.22 (−0.12 to 0.55) 0.36* (−0.02 to 0.73) −0.67 (−2.46 to 1.12) −0.07 (−0.83 to 0.70) −2.57 (−6.23 to 1.08) 0.06 (−0.72 to 0.85) 0.45 (−3.75 to 4.65) 0.08 (−0.26 to 0.43) 0.37 (−3.61 to 4.35) 
 Observations 916 916 829 868 892 892 892 892 892 818 805 

Sample includes children enrolled in the CANDLE Study, Shelby County, Tennessee, 2006–2011. The above estimates represent β coefficients with 95% CIs in parentheses. The effect estimate of interest is the coefficient on the interaction term. Analyses involved regressing each of the outcomes listed above on an indicator for WIC receipt, an indicator for whether the birth occurred after October 2009, and the interaction between the two. The models additionally adjusted for mother’s age, educational attainment, marital status, race, receipt of Medicaid during pregnancy, household size, household income, and total number of pregnancies and child’s sex.

*

P < .10; ** P < .05; *** P < .01.

In this study, we leveraged a quasi-experimental design to provide some of the first evidence of the effects of maternal receipt of the revised WIC food package during pregnancy on developmental outcomes in early childhood. These results suggest that the revised food package resulted in improved length for age at age 12 months and cognitive development at age 24 months.

Maternal nutrition could lead to lasting epigenetic modifications to the fetal genome through a process termed “fetal programming” and may result in developmental adaptations that change the physiology of the offspring in a lasting manner.5355  Previous literature has shown that the WIC revision affected outcomes during several stages of fetal growth and development. Namely, the revised food package was found to improve dietary quality and nutrient intake during pregnancy28,56  as well as downstream perinatal and birth outcomes.32  With our study, we suggest that effects of a mother’s receipt of the WIC revision during pregnancy could be observed into early childhood, up to 24 months of age.

The magnitude of the findings in this study represents clinically relevant effect sizes and provides evidence that one of the largest US safety net policies improves developmental outcomes among low-income and marginalized children. We found a 4.3-point increase in the Bayley cognitive composite score, which represents approximately one-third of an SD. These results are consistent with previous studies, in which researchers found improvements in neurodevelopmental outcomes for children whose families participated in WIC.5759  However, previous work was cross-sectional in nature or evaluated effects of WIC participation more generally rather than the WIC revision. With our study, we advance this understanding by providing quasi-experimental evidence to strengthen the causal interpretation of this association. This provides evidence that policies to improve maternal nutrition during pregnancy can have long-term consequences for child health. Moreover, shifts in the population distribution of Bayley scores, which may be observed in response to far-reaching safety net programs such as WIC, could ultimately lead to reductions in the number of children at risk for developmental delay. We found that there was no association of the revised WIC food package with cognitive scores at ages 4 to 6 years, suggesting that the effects of the revised WIC food package in pregnancy may wane over time. This is also consistent with previous literature suggesting that socioeconomic gaps in cognitive performance widen with age and that even gains in cognitive test scores in response to interventions designed specifically to improve cognitive performance fade over time (although other benefits of early intervention are often observed).60  Furthermore, there are notable measurement differences between the Bayley and SB-5 that reflect large developmental differences between ages 2 to 3 and 4 to 6; correlations between these scores are at best modest within the same, typically developing children.61  Thus, it is possible that improvements in the WIC package do have longer-term impacts on early childhood developmental outcomes not measured in this study.

We additionally found that the revised WIC food package was associated with increased length-for-age z scores at age 12 months by approximately one-fifth of an SD. There is a positive correlation between height in childhood and adulthood,62  so improvements in linear growth could potentially contribute to later life height potentials. Although body height is largely determined by genetic factors, environmental factors including nutrition could explain up to 20% of the height variation in high-income nations.63  There is a well-known association between height in adulthood and higher earnings even in high-income countries,43,44  and height is thought to be a proxy for improved socioeconomic status in childhood more generally. For example, taller children have been found to have higher average cognitive test scores64  and later life educational attainment.44 

There was no observed effect of the revised WIC food package on child growth beyond age 12 months, cognitive development at age 4 to 6 years, or measures of socioemotional developoment for across all age ranges included. There are several potential explanations for why we were unable to detect an association. First, it may be that revisions to the WIC food package were too modest to influence these outcomes, which may be associated with more proximal aspects of the social environment, such as parental stress and mental health.65  Relatedly, child language is associated with the home language environment,66  which may overshadow more distal associations with prenatal nutrition. Second, it may be that effect sizes were too small to detect given of the sample size. Third, pregnant women’s receipt of the modest revision to WIC could have limited benefits to child development over time. Finally, although there are hypothesized mechanisms through which improved maternal nutrition in pregnancy could influence socioemotional development,17,56,67  the current evidence remains correlational and largely inconclusive,6870  and it may be that there is in fact no effect for this particular outcome.

This study has several strengths. We are among the first to test the effects of the revised WIC food package on downstream child health. In this study, we leveraged a natural experiment to produce rigorous estimates of the effects of the revised food package on several outcomes, strengthening the growing body of evidence that supports the positive association between WIC and multiple health outcomes. Furthermore, by examining a variety of outcomes related to child growth and development, in this study we provide a more-comprehensive illustration of the different pathways through which improved maternal nutrition affects child growth and development. Finally, this study was conducted with data from the CANDLE cohort, providing rich longitudinal data on both pregnancy and later child health, which is rare in the United States.

This study has several limitations. Although the WIC food package is standardized at the federal level, food availability and access and the characteristics of women and children in Tennessee may differ from other states. Therefore, results may not be generalizable to other states. However, CANDLE participants share characteristics with other, majority-Black communities in the urban South that are underrepresented in studies of child development and face high levels of social disadvantage. Additionally, income, WIC receipt, and other covariates were self-reported and may suffer from reporting bias. Next, the findings were not uniformly robust to sensitivity analyses, although the sensitivity analyses were not considered to be the main analyses because of their own limitations (eg, the sensitivity analysis using WIC eligibility as the treatment group was subject to measurement error because income was collected as a crude categorical variable). Furthermore, because most sensitivity analyses led to reduced sample size, this may have resulted in reduced power to detect effects. Also, children whose mothers were eligible for WIC in pregnancy may themselves be eligible for WIC after birth, so part of the effect that we are capturing may reflect continued receipt of the revised WIC package during childhood. This is likely to bias the results toward the null, because some children born soon before the revision who technically fall into the control group could then receive the revised WIC food package as children, therefore making them more similar to the treatment group. This also makes it difficult to disentangle the specific aspect of the WIC revision that led to the results found in this study. Additionally, because we evaluate several outcomes, multiple hypothesis testing could have led to a false rejection of the null hypothesis. Finally, we observed potential differences in receipt of Medicaid or TennCare and household size among WIC participants and nonparticipants over time. Although the analyses were adjusted for these factors, we cannot rule out confounding by unmeasured factors. Finally, there may be additional effects of WIC on childhood development (eg, socioemotional development at 4–6 years) that were not evaluated in this current study because violations of the statistical models.

Findings from this study suggest that revisions to WIC, one of the largest United States public health programs, resulted in downstream child health benefits among a high-risk, understudied population. These findings provide timely and critical evidence for the role that WIC plays in improving the health of the nation’s most vulnerable populations, suggesting meaningful impacts of the revised WIC food package on child development. It provides rigorous scientific evidence that the WIC program may be essential to minimizing the burden of poor health experienced by the most socially disadvantaged members of society. Given the substantial reach of WIC among vulnerable populations, this study has implications for future WIC policy decisions and other related safety net programs, especially given the decrease in program funding for this upcoming fiscal year.71,72  Specifically, considering the relatively modest scope of the 2009 revision, more-substantial updates to the program based on up-to-date nutritional guidance may have substantial effects on improving the health of WIC recipients. Additionally, considering that half of eligible pregnant women do not participate in WIC, increasing enrollment may be a useful strategy for reducing the intergenerational transmission of health disparities in communities facing poverty and social disadvantage.

Ms Guan conceptualized this study, conducted the initial analysis, and drafted the initial manuscript; Dr Hamad conceptualized this study, supervised the analysis, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr LeWinn conceptualized this study, designed the data collection instruments and coordinated and supervised data collection, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Ms Batra contributed to data organization, data analysis, and code review; Drs Bush and Tylavsky designed the data collection instruments and coordinated and supervised data collection; and all authors critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content and approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

FUNDING: Funded by the National Institutes of Health (National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute grant K08HL132106, National Institute on Aging grant T32AG049663); the University of California, San Francisco, National Center of Excellence in Women’s Health; and the Urban Child Institute. The funders of this study had no role in study design, data collection, data analysis, data interpretation, or writing of the report. Funded by the National Institutes of Health (NIH).

Bayley

Bayley Scales of Infant Development

BITSEA

Brief Infant Toddler Social Emotional Assessment

CANDLE

Conditions Affecting Neurocognitive Development and Learning in Early Childhood

CI

confidence interval

DiD

difference-in-differences

SB-5

Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales

WIC

Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children

1
Barker
DJP
,
Thornburg
KL
.
The obstetric origins of health for a lifetime
.
Clin Obstet Gynecol
.
2013
;
56
(
3
):
511
519
2
Belkacemi
L
,
Nelson
DM
,
Desai
M
,
Ross
MG
.
Maternal undernutrition influences placental-fetal development
.
Biol Reprod
.
2010
;
83
(
3
):
325
331
3
Cohen
JFW
,
Rifas-Shiman
SL
,
Young
J
,
Oken
E
.
Associations of prenatal and child sugar intake with child cognition
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2018
;
54
(
6
):
727
735
4
Jen
V
,
Erler
NS
,
Tielemans
MJ
, et al
.
Mothers’ intake of sugar-containing beverages during pregnancy and body composition of their children during childhood: the Generation R Study
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2017
;
105
(
4
):
834
841
5
Tielemans
MJ
,
Steegers
EAP
,
Voortman
T
, et al
.
Protein intake during pregnancy and offspring body composition at 6 years: the Generation R Study
.
Eur J Nutr
.
2017
;
56
(
6
):
2151
2160
6
Proctor
SL
,
Kyle
J
,
Fefer
K
,
Lau
QC
.
Examining racial microaggressions, race/ethnicity, gender, and bilingual status with school psychology students: the role of intersectionality
.
Contemp Sch Psychol
.
2018
;
22
(
3
):
355
368
7
Canani
RB
,
Costanzo
MD
,
Leone
L
, et al
.
Epigenetic mechanisms elicited by nutrition in early life
.
Nutr Res Rev
.
2011
;
24
(
2
):
198
205
8
Hanley
B
,
Dijane
J
,
Fewtrell
M
, et al
.
Metabolic imprinting, programming and epigenetics - a review of present priorities and future opportunities
.
Br J Nutr
.
2010
;
104
(
suppl 1
):
S1
S25
9
Lowensohn
RI
,
Stadler
DD
,
Naze
C
.
Current concepts of maternal nutrition
.
Obstet Gynecol Surv
.
2016
;
71
(
7
):
413
426
10
Braveman
PA
,
Egerter
SA
,
Mockenhaupt
RE
.
Broadening the focus: the need to address the social determinants of health
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2011
;
40
(
1
suppl 1
):
S4
S18
11
Karp
RJ
,
Cheng
C
,
Meyers
AF
.
The appearance of discretionary income: influence on the prevalence of under- and over-nutrition
.
Int J Equity Health
.
2005
;
4
(
1
):
10
12
Viswanath
K
,
Bond
K
.
Social determinants and nutrition: reflections on the role of communication
.
J Nutr Educ Behav
.
2007
;
39
(
suppl 2
):
S20
S24
13
Gresham
E
,
Byles
JE
,
Bisquera
A
,
Hure
AJ
.
Effects of dietary interventions on neonatal and infant outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2014
;
100
(
5
):
1298
1321
14
Gresham
E
,
Bisquera
A
,
Byles
JE
,
Hure
AJ
.
Effects of dietary interventions on pregnancy outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis
.
Matern Child Nutr
.
2016
;
12
(
1
):
5
23
15
Grieger
JA
,
Clifton
VL
.
A review of the impact of dietary intakes in human pregnancy on infant birthweight
.
Nutrients
.
2014
;
7
(
1
):
153
178
16
Nyaradi
A
,
Li
J
,
Hickling
S
,
Foster
J
,
Oddy
WH
.
The role of nutrition in children’s neurocognitive development, from pregnancy through childhood
.
Front Hum Neurosci
.
2013
;
7
:
97
17
Black
RE
,
Victora
CG
,
Walker
SP
, et al.;
Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group
.
Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries [published correction appears in Lancet. 2013;382(9890):396]
.
Lancet
.
2013
;
382
(
9890
):
427
451
18
US Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service
.
WIC eligibility requirements.
2018
. Available at: https://www.fns.usda.gov/wic/wic-eligibility-requirements. Accessed February 28, 2020
19
Oliveira
VJ
.
The WIC Program: Background, Trends, and Economic Issues
.
Darby, PA
:
DIANE Publishing
;
2009
20
US Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service
.
WIC data tables, national summary.
2020
. Available at: https://www.fns.usda.gov/pd/wic-program. Accessed February 28, 2020
21
Geller
DM
,
Harrington
M
,
Huang
G
.
National Survey of WIC Participants II: Participant Characteristics Report
.
Alexandria, VA
:
US Department of Agriculture, Food and Nutrition Service, Office of Research and Analysis
;
2012
22
Institute of Medicine
;
Food and Nutrition Board
;
Committee to Review the WIC Food Packages
.
WIC Food Packages: Time for a Change
.
Washington, DC
:
National Academy of Sciences
;
2019
23
Yaktine
AL
,
Murphy
SP
.
Aligning nutrition assistance programs with the Dietary Guidelines for Americans
.
Nutr Rev
.
2013
;
71
(
9
):
622
630
24
Zenk
SN
,
Odoms-Young
A
,
Powell
LM
, et al
.
Fruit and vegetable availability and selection: federal food package revisions, 2009
.
Am J Prev Med
.
2012
;
43
(
4
):
423
428
25
Cobb
LK
,
Anderson
CAM
,
Appel
L
, et al
.
Baltimore city stores increased the availability of healthy food after WIC policy change
.
Health Aff (Millwood)
.
2015
;
34
(
11
):
1849
1857
26
Andreyeva
T
,
Luedicke
J
,
Middleton
AE
,
Long
MW
,
Schwartz
MB
.
Positive influence of the revised Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children food packages on access to healthy foods
.
J Acad Nutr Diet
.
2012
;
112
(
6
):
850
858
27
Lu
W
,
McKyer
ELJ
,
Dowdy
D
, et al
.
Evaluating the influence of the revised special supplemental nutrition program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) food allocation package on healthy food availability, accessibility, and affordability in Texas
.
J Acad Nutr Diet
.
2016
;
116
(
2
):
292
301
28
Hamad
R
,
Batra
A
,
Karasek
D
, et al
.
The impact of the revised WIC food package on maternal nutrition during pregnancy and postpartum
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2019
;
188
(
8
):
1493
1502
29
Schultz
DJ
,
Byker Shanks
C
,
Houghtaling
B
.
The impact of the 2009 special supplemental nutrition program for women, infants, and children food package revisions on participants: a systematic review
.
J Acad Nutr Diet
.
2015
;
115
(
11
):
1832
1846
30
Odoms-Young
AM
,
Kong
A
,
Schiffer
LA
, et al
.
Evaluating the initial impact of the revised Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC) food packages on dietary intake and home food availability in African-American and Hispanic families
.
Public Health Nutr
.
2014
;
17
(
1
):
83
93
31
Tester
JM
,
Leung
CW
,
Crawford
PB
.
Revised WIC food package and children’s diet quality
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
137
(
5
):
e20153557
32
Hamad
R
,
Collin
DF
,
Baer
RJ
,
Jelliffe-Pawlowski
LL
.
Association of revised WIC food package with perinatal and birth outcomes: a quasi-experimental study
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2019
;
173
(
9
):
845
852
33
Li
K
,
Wen
M
,
Reynolds
M
,
Zhang
Q
.
WIC participation and breastfeeding after the 2009 WIC revision: a propensity score approach
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2019
;
16
(
15
):
2645
34
Langellier
BA
,
Chaparro
MP
,
Wang
MC
,
Koleilat
M
,
Whaley
SE
.
The new food package and breastfeeding outcomes among women, infants, and children participants in Los Angeles County
.
Am J Public Health
.
2014
;
104
(
suppl 1
):
S112
S118
35
Wilde
P
,
Wolf
A
,
Fernandes
M
,
Collins
A
.
Food-package assignments and breastfeeding initiation before and after a change in the Special Supplemental Nutrition Program for Women, Infants, and Children
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2012
;
96
(
3
):
560
566
36
Sontag-Padilla
L
,
Burns
R
,
Shih
R
, et al
.
The Urban Child Institute CANDLE Study: Methodological Overview and Baseline Sample Description
.
Santa Monica, CA
:
RAND Corporation
;
2015
37
Food and Nutrition Services (FNS)
;
USDA
.
Final rule. “Special supplemental nutrition program for Women, Infants, and Children (WIC): revisions in the WIC food packages.” Part II
.
Fed Regist
.
2014
;
79
(
42
):
12274
12300
38
Grummer-Strawn
LM
,
Reinold
C
,
Krebs
NF
;
Centers for Disease control and Prevention (CDC)
.
Use of World Health Organization and CDC growth charts for children aged 0-59 months in the United States [published correction appears in MMWR Recomm Rep. 2010;59(36):1184]
.
MMWR Recomm Rep
.
2010
;
59
(
RR-9
):
1
15
39
Rudolf
MCJ
,
Logan
S
.
What is the long term outcome for children who fail to thrive? A systematic review
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2005
;
90
(
9
):
925
931
40
Corbett
SS
,
Drewett
RF
.
To what extent is failure to thrive in infancy associated with poorer cognitive development? A review and meta-analysis
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2004
;
45
(
3
):
641
654
41
Cole
TJ
,
Wright
CM
.
A chart to predict adult height from a child’s current height
.
Ann Hum Biol
.
2011
;
38
(
6
):
662
668
42
Binkin
NJ
,
Yip
R
,
Fleshood
L
,
Trowbridge
FL
.
Birth weight and childhood growth
.
Pediatrics
.
1988
;
82
(
6
):
828
834
43
Böckerman
P
,
Viinikainen
J
,
Vainiomäki
J
, et al
.
Stature and long-term labor market outcomes: evidence using Mendelian randomization
.
Econ Hum Biol
.
2017
;
24
:
18
29
44
Rietveld
CA
,
Hessels
J
,
van der Zwan
P
.
The stature of the self-employed and its relation with earnings and satisfaction
.
Econ Hum Biol
.
2015
;
17
:
59
74
45
Briggs-Gowan
MJ
,
Carter
AS
,
Irwin
JR
,
Wachtel
K
,
Cicchetti
DV
.
The Brief Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment: screening for social-emotional problems and delays in competence
.
J Pediatr Psychol
.
2004
;
29
(
2
):
143
155
46
Bayley
N
.
Bayley Scales of Infant Development and Toddler Development: Administration Manual
, 3rd ed.
San Antonio, TX
:
PsychCorp, Harcourt Assessment
;
2006
47
Månsson
J
,
Stjernqvist
K
,
Serenius
F
,
Ådén
U
,
Källén
K
.
Agreement between bayley-III measurements and WISC-IV measurements in typically developing children
.
J Psychoed Assess
.
2019
;
37
(
5
):
603
616
48
Patrianakos-Hoobler
AI
,
Msall
ME
,
Huo
D
,
Marks
JD
,
Plesha-Troyke
S
,
Schreiber
MD
.
Predicting school readiness from neurodevelopmental assessments at age 2 years after respiratory distress syndrome in infants born preterm
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2010
;
52
(
4
):
379
385
49
Bode
MM
,
DʼEugenio
DB
,
Mettelman
BB
,
Gross
SJ
. Predictive Validity of the Bayley, Third Edition at 2 Years for Intelligence Quotient at 4 Years in Preterm Infants. In:
J Dev Behav Pediatr
, vol.
35
.
2014
:
570
575
50
Roid
GH
,
Pomplun
M
. Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues. In:
Flanagan
DP
,
Harrison
PL
, eds.
The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales
.
New York, NY
:
The Guilford Press
;
2012
:
249
268
51
Dimick
JB
,
Ryan
AM
.
Methods for evaluating changes in health care policy: the difference-in-differences approach
.
JAMA
.
2014
;
312
(
22
):
2401
2402
52
US Department of Agriculture
.
WIC 2016 eligibility and coverage rates.
2019
. Available at: https://www.fns.usda.gov/wic/wic-2016-eligibility-and-coverage-rates. Accessed February 28, 2020
53
Indrio
F
,
Martini
S
,
Francavilla
R
, et al
.
Epigenetic matters: the link between early nutrition, microbiome, and long-term health development
.
Front Pediatr
.
2017
;
5
:
178
54
Mühlhäusler
BS
,
Adam
CL
,
McMillen
IC
.
Maternal nutrition and the programming of obesity: the brain
.
Organogenesis
.
2008
;
4
(
3
):
144
152
55
Wu
G
,
Bazer
FW
,
Cudd
TA
,
Meininger
CJ
,
Spencer
TE
.
Maternal nutrition and fetal development
.
J Nutr
.
2004
;
134
(
9
):
2169
2172
56
Whaley
SE
,
Ritchie
LD
,
Spector
P
,
Gomez
J
.
Revised WIC food package improves diets of WIC families
.
J Nutr Educ Behav
.
2012
;
44
(
3
):
204
209
57
Lakshmanan
A
,
Song
AY
,
Flores-Fenlon
N
, et al
.
Association of WIC participation and growth and developmental outcomes in high-risk infants
.
Clin Pediatr (Phila)
.
2020
;
59
(
1
):
53
61
58
Jackson
MI
.
Early childhood WIC participation, cognitive development and academic achievement
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2015
;
126
:
145
153
59
Bolbocean
C
,
Tylavsky
FA
,
West
JE
.
U.S. Safety Net Programs and Early Life Skills Formation: Results from a Prospective Longitudinal Cohort Study
.
Cambridge, MA
:
National Bureau of Economic Research, Inc
;
2018
60
von Stumm
S
,
Plomin
R
.
Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence
.
Intelligence
.
2015
;
48
:
30
36
61
Kamppi
D
,
Gilmore
L
. Assessing Cognitive Development in Early Childhood: A Comparison of the Bayley-III and the Stanford-Binet, Fifth Edition. In:
Aust Educ Dev Psychol
, vol.
27
.
2010
:
70
75
62
Power
C
,
Lake
JK
,
Cole
TJ
.
Body mass index and height from childhood to adulthood in the 1958 British born cohort
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
1997
;
66
(
5
):
1094
1101
63
Silventoinen
K
.
Determinants of variation in adult body height
.
J Biosoc Sci
.
2003
;
35
(
2
):
263
285
64
Case
A
,
Paxson
C
.
Stature and status: height, ability, and labor market outcomes
.
J Polit Econ
.
2008
;
116
(
3
):
499
532
65
Palmer
FB
,
Anand
KJS
,
Graff
JC
, et al
.
Early adversity, socioemotional development, and stress in urban 1-year-old children
.
J Pediatr
.
2013
;
163
(
6
):
1733
1739.e1
66
Huttenlocher
J
,
Vasilyeva
M
,
Cymerman
E
,
Levine
S
.
Language input and child syntax
.
Cognit Psychol
.
2002
;
45
(
3
):
337
374
67
Bellisle
F
.
Effects of diet on behaviour and cognition in children
.
Br J Nutr
.
2004
;
92
(
suppl 2
):
S227
S232
68
Arons
A
,
Bolbocean
C
,
Bush
NR
,
Tylavsky
FA
,
LeWinn
KZ
.
Participation in the special supplemental nutrition program for women, infants, and children is not associated with early childhood socioemotional development: results from a longitudinal cohort study
.
Prev Med Rep
.
2016
;
4
:
507
511
69
Rush
D
,
Sloan
NL
,
Leighton
J
, et al
.
The national WIC evaluation: evaluation of the special supplemental food program for women, infants, and children. V. Longitudinal study of pregnant women
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
1988
;
48
(
suppl 2
):
439
483
70.
Kowaleski-Jones
L
,
Duncan
GJ
.
Effects of participation in the WIC food assistance program on children’s health and development: evidence from NLSY children.
Madison, WI: Wisconsin University, Madison Institute for Research on Poverty. Available at: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED445158. Accessed February 20, 2020
71
National WIC Association
.
House Unveils FY 2021 Appropriations Bills, Including WIC Funding
.
Washington, DC
:
National WIC Association
;
2020
72
National WIC Association
.
White House Releases President’s Budget for Fiscal Year 2021
.
Washington, DC
:
National WIC Association
;
2020

Competing Interests

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Supplementary data