BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES

Children born very preterm (<32 weeks’ gestation) have more neurodevelopmental problems compared with term-born peers. Aberrant fidgety movements (FMs) are associated with adverse motor outcomes in children born very preterm. However, associations of aberrant FMs combined with additional movements and postures to give a motor optimality score-revised (MOS-R) with school-aged cognitive and motor outcomes are unclear. Our aim with this study was to determine those associations.

METHODS

Of 118 infants born <30 weeks’ gestation recruited into a randomized controlled trial of early intervention, 97 had a general movements assessment at 3 months’ corrected age and were eligible for this study. Early motor repertoire including FMs and MOS-R were scored from videos of infant’s spontaneous movement at 3 months’ corrected age. At 8 years’ corrected age, cognitive and motor performances were evaluated. Associations of early FMs and MOS-R with outcomes at 8 years were determined using linear regression.

RESULTS

Seventy-eight (80%) infants with early motor repertoire data had neurodevelopmental assessments at 8 years. A higher MOS-R, and favorable components of the individual subscales of the MOS-R, including the presence of normal FMs, were associated with better performance for general cognition, attention, working memory, executive function and motor function at 8 years; eg, presence of normal FMs was associated with a 21.6 points higher general conceptual ability score (95% confidence interval: 12.8–30.5; P < .001) compared with absent FMs.

CONCLUSIONS

Favorable early motor repertoire of infants born <30 weeks is strongly associated with improved cognitive and motor performance at 8 years.

What’s known on This Subject:

In children born very preterm, the General Movements Assessment ∼3 months' corrected age, known as the assessment of fidgety movements, is predictive of various neurodevelopment domains in early childhood.

What This Study Adds:

Favorable early motor repertoire ∼3 months’ corrected age, involving fidgety movements, along with observed movement patterns, age-appropriate repertoire, postural patterns, and movement character, is associated with better cognitive and motor function in 8-year-old children who were born very preterm.

Motor and cognitive impairments are common among children born very preterm (VP; <32 weeks’ gestation).1  Motor impairments can be in the form of cerebral palsy (CP) or developmental coordination disorder.1,2  Cognitive impairments may be reflected as learning disabilities or problems with attention, memory, learning, visual-motor integration, language, or problem solving and can vary in severity.3,4  Although numerous perinatal variables, such as gestational age at birth and birth weight, have been related to neurodevelopmental outcome, these variables alone do not explain all poorer motor or cognitive function in children born VP.2,5 

An increasingly used method related with neurodevelopmental outcome is the assessment of the early motor repertoire with Prechtl’s General Movements Assessment.6  When using this method, infants’ spontaneous movements up to 20 weeks’ corrected age can be scored. At ∼11 to 20 weeks, so-called fidgety movements (FMs) occur. Normal FMs are continuous small-amplitude, moderate-speed movements of shoulders, wrists, hips, and ankles in all directions and of variable accelerations. Other than normal, they can be judged as aberrant (abnormal or absent).7  Absence of FMs is considered worse than abnormal FMs. FMs can be accompanied by a range of other movements and postures. Together they compose a quantitative measure for the integrity of the infants’ central nervous system: the motor optimality score-revised (MOS-R).6,7  Most early motor repertoire studies have been focused on FMs alone and their associations with later motor development. FMs have been reported to have a sensitivity of 98% for predicting CP on the basis of several systematic reviews.7,8  We have previously reported that the absence of FMs in VP infants was associated with poorer motor and cognitive outcome at both 2 and 4 years.9  In various groups of children, the overall MOS-R and aspects of its subscales have been related to cognition,10  the severity of CP,7  and the severity of developmental coordination disorder.11  The aim with the current study was to determine if FMs and other aspects of the early motor repertoire are associated with school-aged motor or cognitive outcomes in children born <30 weeks’ gestation. It was hypothesized that favorable early movement repertoire, including FMs and other components of the MOS-R, would be associated with better cognitive and motor performance at school-age.

One hundred and eighteen infants born <30 weeks’ gestation between January 2005 and September 2006 and admitted to the Royal Women’s Hospital, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia, participated in an early intervention trial over the first 12 months after birth.12  Survivors were invited for follow-up assessments at 2, 4, and 8 years’ corrected age (Fig 1). Because no substantial differences in motor and cognitive outcomes related to the intervention were found at 2, 4 and 8 years,9,13,14  groups were combined for the current study, which is focused on the 8-year outcomes. The Royal Women’s Hospital and Royal Children’s Hospital Human Research Ethics Committees approved the study. Written informed consent was obtained from all parents. Families were classified as lower or higher social risk on the basis of 6 aspects of social status, as previously reported.15 

FIGURE 1

Inclusion flowchart. GM, general movement.

FIGURE 1

Inclusion flowchart. GM, general movement.

Close modal

At 3 months’ corrected age, 20- to 30-minute video recordings of spontaneous movements were made in 97 infants. The recordings were scored according to Prechtl’s method by 2 independent assessors who were blinded to the infants’ clinical history and had completed formal training with the General Movement Trust (90.9% agreement; Cohen’s ĸ = 0.77, SE = 0.21), with the MOS-R completed by 1 assessor.6,13,16  An infant’s FMs were classified as normal when continuous small-amplitude, moderate-speed movements of shoulders, wrists, hips, and ankles in all directions and of variable accelerations were seen. In case of an exaggeration of movements in terms of speed and amplitude, FMs were judged abnormal. If FMs were not observed, they were judged as absent.16  In addition, the MOS-R was determined on the basis of the following subscales: observed movement patterns, age-adequacy, observed postural patterns, and movement character. Each subscale consists of 3 categories on the basis of which scores of 1, 2 or 4 were given, in ascending order of normality. FMs are an exception and scores of 1 (absent), 4 (abnormal), and 12 (normal) were used. As a result, a MOS-R ranging from 5 to 28 points was possible, with higher scores reflecting better performance.6,7,11,17 

At 8 years, children were administered a test battery assessing various neurocognitive developmental domains.18 

General Cognition

To assess general cognition, the Differential Ability Scale - Second edition (DAS-II) was administered. Indices of interest include the General Conceptual Ability scale, which is a composite scale from the verbal, nonverbal, and spatial reasoning scales.19  A normative mean of 100 (SD = 15) applies to all scales.

Attention

Attention was assessed by using the Score! (sustained attention) and Sky Search (selective attention) subtests of the Test of Everyday Attention for Children, which have a mean of 10 (SD = 3).20 

Working Memory

Working memory was assessed by using subtests from the Working Memory Test Battery-Children. Digit Recall was used to assess immediate verbal memory, whereas Backward Digit Recall was used to assess verbal working memory. A normative mean of 100 (SD = 15) applies to both subtests.21 

Executive Function

The Tower of London was administered to assess planning ability. The measure of interest was the number of items correctly solved within 60 seconds.22 

Motor

Motor outcome was determined by using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-II. A total score and scores for manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and balance tasks were obtained. A normative mean of 10 applies to all scales (SD = 3).23 

Data were analyzed by using SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 23 (IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM Corporation). Baseline characteristics of participating children were compared with those lost to follow-up. Statistical tests used were independent t test, Mann-Whitney U test, χ2 test, and Fisher’s exact test. Associations between the various neurodevelopmental outcomes and the total score on the MOS-R and with the subscales were calculated by using linear regression analysis. The subscales FMs, observed movement patterns, observed postural patterns, and movement character were analyzed as normal versus the remainder and coded as 1 or 0, respectively. The subscale age-adequate repertoire was analyzed based on the 3 categories “present,” “reduced,” and “absent” and coded as 2, 1, or 0, respectively. To correct for multiple testing, a false discovery rate of 5% was applied. Multivariable linear regression analysis was used when covariates (gestational age, z scores of birth weight, presence of white matter injury, and social status) were incorporated.

At 8 years, 78 of 97 (80%) of those with general movements at 3 months from the initial cohort participated in neurodevelopmental follow-up assessments. There was evidence that children who were assessed at 8 years had a higher birth weight than those who were not but little evidence of a difference in proportions who were extremely low birth weight between the groups. (Table 1). Furthermore, there was evidence that participating mothers more often received antenatal corticosteroids than nonparticipating mothers. The was little evidence for differences in other patient characteristics between groups. Follow-up assessments were performed at a mean corrected age of 8.0 (0.7 SD) years.

TABLE 1

Patient Characteristics of Children Participating in Childhood Compared With Remaining Children of the Original Sample

Participants in Childhood Follow-up, n = 78Remaining Sample in Infancy, n = 19P
Gestational age, mean ± SD, wk 27.4 ± 1.4 26.8 ± 1.8 .14 
Birth wt, mean ± SD, g 1048 ± 261 884 ± 261 .016* 
Gestational age <28 wk 41 (53) 13 (68) .21 
Birth wt <1000 g, n (%) 38 (49) 12 (63) .26 
Male, n (%) 43 (55) 7 (37) .15 
Twins, n (%) 7 (9) 2 (11) .83 
Antenatal corticosteroids, n (%) 70 (90) 13 (68) .018* 
Apgar score 5 min, median (25th, 75th percentile) 8 (7, 9) 8 (5, 9) .16 
Oxygen at 36 wk, n (%) 26 (33) 6 (32) .88 
IVH grade ≥III, n (%) 7 (9) 1 (5) .60 
WMI grade ≥III, n (%) 8 (10) 2 (11) .97 
Postnatal corticosteroids, n (%) 4 (5) 1 (5) .98 
Higher social risk,an (%) 30 (39) 11 (58) .12 
CP, n (%) 4 (5) 1 (5) .98 
Participants in Childhood Follow-up, n = 78Remaining Sample in Infancy, n = 19P
Gestational age, mean ± SD, wk 27.4 ± 1.4 26.8 ± 1.8 .14 
Birth wt, mean ± SD, g 1048 ± 261 884 ± 261 .016* 
Gestational age <28 wk 41 (53) 13 (68) .21 
Birth wt <1000 g, n (%) 38 (49) 12 (63) .26 
Male, n (%) 43 (55) 7 (37) .15 
Twins, n (%) 7 (9) 2 (11) .83 
Antenatal corticosteroids, n (%) 70 (90) 13 (68) .018* 
Apgar score 5 min, median (25th, 75th percentile) 8 (7, 9) 8 (5, 9) .16 
Oxygen at 36 wk, n (%) 26 (33) 6 (32) .88 
IVH grade ≥III, n (%) 7 (9) 1 (5) .60 
WMI grade ≥III, n (%) 8 (10) 2 (11) .97 
Postnatal corticosteroids, n (%) 4 (5) 1 (5) .98 
Higher social risk,an (%) 30 (39) 11 (58) .12 
CP, n (%) 4 (5) 1 (5) .98 

IVH, intraventricular hemorrhage; WMI, white matter injury.

a

Social risk status was determined on the basis of 6 aspects of social status as reported previously.15 

*

P ≤.05.

Most children had normal FMs, a higher number of normal than abnormal observed movement patterns, higher number of normal than abnormal observed postural patterns, and smooth and fluent movement character, but only a minority had an age-adequate repertoire. (Table 2). The median MOS-R was 23.5 (25th and 75th percentile: 23.0, 28.0).

TABLE 2

Early Motor Repertoire Scores

Classificationan = 78
FMs, No. (%)  
 Normal (12) 68 (87) 
 Abnormal (4) 0 (0) 
 Absent (1) 10 (13) 
Observed movement patterns, No. (%)  
 N>A (4) 68 (87) 
 N=A (2) 7 (9) 
 N<A (1) 3 (4) 
Age-adequate repertoire, No. (%)  
 Present (4) 24 (31) 
 Reduced (2) 28 (36) 
 Absent (1) 26 (33) 
Observed postural patterns, No. (%)  
 N>A (4) 62 (80) 
 N=A (2) 14 (18) 
 N<A (1) 2 (2) 
Movement character, No. (%)  
 Smooth and fluent (4) 52 (67) 
 Abnormal, not cramped: synchronized (2) 26 (33) 
 Cramped: synchronized (1) 0 (0) 
MOS-R, median (25th, 75th percentile) 23.5 (23.0, 28.0) 
Classificationan = 78
FMs, No. (%)  
 Normal (12) 68 (87) 
 Abnormal (4) 0 (0) 
 Absent (1) 10 (13) 
Observed movement patterns, No. (%)  
 N>A (4) 68 (87) 
 N=A (2) 7 (9) 
 N<A (1) 3 (4) 
Age-adequate repertoire, No. (%)  
 Present (4) 24 (31) 
 Reduced (2) 28 (36) 
 Absent (1) 26 (33) 
Observed postural patterns, No. (%)  
 N>A (4) 62 (80) 
 N=A (2) 14 (18) 
 N<A (1) 2 (2) 
Movement character, No. (%)  
 Smooth and fluent (4) 52 (67) 
 Abnormal, not cramped: synchronized (2) 26 (33) 
 Cramped: synchronized (1) 0 (0) 
MOS-R, median (25th, 75th percentile) 23.5 (23.0, 28.0) 

N<A, lower number of normal than abnormal; N=A, same number of normal and abnormal; N>A, higher number of normal than abnormal.

a

Scores for all subscale levels are presented in parentheses.

Scores on neurodevelopmental tests are presented in Table 3. Four children diagnosed with CP earlier in childhood were unable to complete all tests because of their physical impairment.

TABLE 3

Neurodevelopmental Scores

All Participants in Childhood Follow-up, n = 78Excluding Children with CP, n = 74
Age at assessment in y, mean ± SD 8.0 ± 0.7 8.0 ± 0.7 
General cognitiona   
 General conceptual ability 100.5 ± 13.6 101.5 ± 12.8 
 Verbal composite 100.2 ± 12.6 101.1 ± 11.7 
 Nonverbal composite 96.3 ± 13.6 97.2 ± 12.8 
 Spatial reasoning composite 103.8 ± 16.5 105.1 ± 15.3 
Attentionb   
 Sustained attention 7.2 ± 3.7 7.3 ± 3.6 
 Selective attention 8.8 ± 3.9 9.1 ± 3.7 
Working memorya   
 Immediate verbal memory 100.6 ± 20.5 100.9 ± 20.4 
 Verbal working memory 94.2 ± 16.5 94.6 ± 16.4 
Executive functionc   
 Spatial planning and behavioral inhibition 9.6 ± 1.6 9.6 ± 1.6 
Motorb   
 Total 8.2 ± 3.7 8.2 ± 3.7 
 Manual dexterity 8.1 ± 3.7 8.2 ± 3.7 
 Aiming and catching 9.5 ± 3.0 9.5 ± 3.1 
 Balance 8.3 ± 3.8 8.4 ± 3.7 
All Participants in Childhood Follow-up, n = 78Excluding Children with CP, n = 74
Age at assessment in y, mean ± SD 8.0 ± 0.7 8.0 ± 0.7 
General cognitiona   
 General conceptual ability 100.5 ± 13.6 101.5 ± 12.8 
 Verbal composite 100.2 ± 12.6 101.1 ± 11.7 
 Nonverbal composite 96.3 ± 13.6 97.2 ± 12.8 
 Spatial reasoning composite 103.8 ± 16.5 105.1 ± 15.3 
Attentionb   
 Sustained attention 7.2 ± 3.7 7.3 ± 3.6 
 Selective attention 8.8 ± 3.9 9.1 ± 3.7 
Working memorya   
 Immediate verbal memory 100.6 ± 20.5 100.9 ± 20.4 
 Verbal working memory 94.2 ± 16.5 94.6 ± 16.4 
Executive functionc   
 Spatial planning and behavioral inhibition 9.6 ± 1.6 9.6 ± 1.6 
Motorb   
 Total 8.2 ± 3.7 8.2 ± 3.7 
 Manual dexterity 8.1 ± 3.7 8.2 ± 3.7 
 Aiming and catching 9.5 ± 3.0 9.5 ± 3.1 
 Balance 8.3 ± 3.8 8.4 ± 3.7 
a

Normative mean of 100 (SD = 15).

b

Normative mean of 10 (SD = 3).

c

Tower of London N correct in 60 s (raw scores) with maximum of 12.

General Cognition

There was strong evidence for positive associations between a higher score on the MOS-R itself, as well as on all of its subscales, with better scores for all DAS-II composites including verbal, nonverbal, spatial reasoning, and General Conceptual Ability scores (Fig 2). After excluding 4 children with CP, the evidence for all associations remained except for those between observed postural patterns and verbal and nonverbal reasoning (Supplemental Table 4). When adjusted for covariates (ie gestational age, z scores of birth weight, presence of white matter injury, and social status), the evidence for positive associations remained strong, except for the association between the subscale observed postural patterns and verbal and nonverbal reasoning (Fig 2).

FIGURE 2

Associations between the early motor repertoire and cognition. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable); GCA, General Conceptual Ability.

FIGURE 2

Associations between the early motor repertoire and cognition. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable); GCA, General Conceptual Ability.

Close modal

Attention

For a higher MOS-R, as well as for higher scores on observed movement patterns, there was strong evidence for associations with better sustained and selective attention (Fig 3). For the other MOS-R subscales, results were more heterogeneous. There was strong evidence that children who had normal FMs in infancy had better sustained attention than their peers in whom FMs had been absent, but no evidence for differences in selective attention was found. Furthermore, evidence was found that better performance on the age-adequate repertoire and observed postural patterns subscales were associated with higher scores for selective attention but not for sustained attention. After excluding children with CP and after adjusting for covariates, evidence for the aforementioned associations remained (Supplemental Table 4; Fig 3).

FIGURE 3

Associations between the early motor repertoire and attention. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. Unadjusted association between movement character and sustained attention lost its significance after false discovery rate correction. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

FIGURE 3

Associations between the early motor repertoire and attention. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. Unadjusted association between movement character and sustained attention lost its significance after false discovery rate correction. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

Close modal

Working Memory

There was strong evidence for positive relations between the MOS-R and all its subscales except observed postural pattern with verbal memory and verbal working memory (Fig 4). When children with CP were excluded, the evidence weakened for the associations between the quality of FMs and verbal working memory as well as observed movement patterns and verbal working memory (Supplemental Table 4). After adjusting for covariates, the associations between the quality of FMs and both memory test scores weakened (Fig 4).

FIGURE 4

Associations between the early motor repertoire and working memory. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

FIGURE 4

Associations between the early motor repertoire and working memory. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

Close modal

Executive Function

There was evidence that higher scores on the subscales of observed movement patterns and movement character were related to better performance on the spatial planning and behavioral inhibition task (Fig 5). There was little evidence for associations of the MOS-R, FMs, age-adequate repertoire, or observed postural patterns subscales with performance on the spatial planning and behavioral inhibition task, before and after excluding children with CP, and after adjusting for covariates (Supplemental Table 4; Fig 5).

FIGURE 5

Associations between the early motor repertoire and executive function. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. Note: unadjusted association between MOS-R and executive function lost its significance after false discovery rate correction. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

FIGURE 5

Associations between the early motor repertoire and executive function. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. Note: unadjusted association between MOS-R and executive function lost its significance after false discovery rate correction. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

Close modal

Motor

There was strong evidence that a higher MOS-R, presence of normal FMs, presence of an age-adequate repertoire, and a smooth and fluent movement character were associated not only with better total motor performance but also with its components of manual dexterity, aiming and catching, and balance (Fig 6). After excluding children with CP and after adjustment for covariates, the strength of the evidence for the relationships remained strong (Supplemental Table 4; Fig 6).

FIGURE 6

Associations between the early motor repertoire and motor. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. Note: unadjusted association between postural patterns and manual dexterity lost its significance after false discovery rate correction. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

FIGURE 6

Associations between the early motor repertoire and motor. A, B, D, and E: analyzed as normal versus the remainder; C: analyzed on the basis of 3 categories. Note: unadjusted association between postural patterns and manual dexterity lost its significance after false discovery rate correction. B, unstandardized coefficient (represents change in dependent variable per unit change in independent variable).

Close modal

In this study, we demonstrate that the early motor repertoire ∼3 months’ corrected age is strongly associated with many areas of neurodevelopment at 8 years in children born <30 weeks’ gestational age, including cognition, attention, working memory, executive function, and motor function. It is consistent with findings of previous follow-up measurements in the same cohort that studied the association between FMs only and neurodevelopmental outcome up until 4 years. Unique to the current report is that it extends those positive associations to aspects of the MOS-R other than just FMs, and also into school-age.

Better performance on all subscales of the MOS-R, as well as the total MOS-R, were associated with higher scaled scores for general cognition at 8 years (eg, children with normal FMs had higher scores by 17 to 25 points compared with children who did not have normal FMs). In our cohort at the 2- and 4 year follow-up time points, group differences for these scaled scores were only 7.4 (95% confidence interval [CI]: 0.9–13.9) and 14.3 (95% CI: 5.1–23.9), respectively.9  This widening group difference might be attributed to the ongoing emergence of specific cognitive skills. In addition, it should be noted that at 2 years, cognition was tested using the Bayley Scales of Infant Development-III, which is known to overestimate neurodevelopmental performance.24 

We found evidence that selective and sustained attention were associated with the MOS-R and various subscales. Researchers in 2 studies who addressed the same associations in preterm and healthy term infants did not provide any conclusive evidence.25,26  Further research is needed to identify associations with attention in other groups of infants.

Better immediate and working memory were associated with a higher MOS-R and higher scores on all subscales, except observed postural patterns. This is consistent with findings of Grunewaldt et al, who found that extremely low birth weight children with an abnormal movement character in infancy had a poorer working memory at 10 years compared with those who had a normal movement character.27 

Evidence was found that better performance on the spatial planning and behavioral inhibition task was associated with higher scores for observed movement patterns and movement character. Compared with the other neurodevelopmental domains investigated in our study, this subdomain of executive function was less associated with the early motor repertoire. Research into associations of early motor repertoire with other domains of executive function is required.

Favorable motor performance was strongly associated with a higher MOS-R and higher scores on most subscales. Our findings are consistent with those of Bruggink et al, who reported that in preterm children, the combination of normal FMs with an abnormal movement character increases the risk for developmental coordination disorder.28  Fjørtoft et al came to a similar conclusion on the basis of a study in infants who were at high-risk for poor neurodevelopment. They also found that a poor motor outcome could be identified by an abnormal movement character in the presence of normal FMs at 14 weeks’ corrected age.29 

Although group means for the whole cohort are close to expected values, they are performing worse than Australian children who were not born preterm. When we assessed the cohort at 4 years of age, they were 6 to 10 points below term-born children in the control group on DAS-II composite scales.18  The reductions in cognitive scores for those with abnormalities on the GMA in the current study put them further into impaired ranges compared with the majority of children in Australia, and hence our findings are clinically important.

Most associations remained strong after excluding 4 children with CP and after adjusting for perinatal and social factors, which implies that the associations between the early motor repertoire and neurodevelopment in VP children are robust.

Our findings may be explained by neuronal changes occurring in infancy. Approximately 2 to 3 months’ corrected age, neuronal activity of the cortex, cerebellar cortex, and basal ganglia increases markedly.30  At the same time, the early motor repertoire with age-specific spontaneous movements and postures emerges. This early motor repertoire is considered to reflect the integrity of the central nervous system. Based on that, any disruption to the central nervous system may result in abnormalities in the early motor repertoire.6,31  Because VP infants are at risk for perinatal brain alterations such as white matter injury that often results in poor neurodevelopment,13,32  studying their early motor repertoire may help in predicting their neurodevelopmental outcome.

The broad range of neurodevelopmental domains that were tested in childhood, using validated and normed tests, was a strength of our study. The follow-up was ∼80%, which was high considering the infants were initially recruited at birth with no school-age follow-up planned. A limitation is the inability to account for all factors influencing and interfering with neurodevelopment up until 8 years. However, we were able to allow for some important perinatal variables and the child’s social risk status.

Favorable early motor repertoire at 3 months’ corrected age is strongly associated with better neurodevelopment at 8 years in VP children. These findings indicate that the early motor repertoire may be useful in identifying infants at risk for poor neurodevelopment.

We acknowledge Merilyn Bear, research nurse, who has coordinated recruitment and follow-up of this cohort.

Dr Salavati and Profs Bos and Spittle conceived and designed the study, analyzed and interpreted the data, and drafted and revised the manuscript; Profs Anderson and Doyle assisted with the overall study design and implementation, interpreted the results, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

FUNDING: Funded by grants from the National Health and Medical Council (Project Grant identifier [ID] 284512; Investigator Grant [P.J.A.] ID 1176077; Career Development Fellowship [A.J.S.] ID 1159533; Centre of Research Excellence ID 1060733 and 1153176); The Cerebral Palsy Alliance Project Grant; Murdoch Childrens Research Institute, Myer Foundation, Allens Arthur Robinson Foundation, Thyne Reid Foundation, and the Victorian Government’s Operational Infrastructure Support Program. The funders did not participate in the work.

CI

confidence interval

CP

cerebral palsy

DAS-II

Differential Ability Scale

FM

fidgety movement

MOS-R

motor optimality score-revised

VP

very preterm

1
van Baar
AL
,
van Wassenaer
AG
,
Briët
JM
,
Dekker
FW
,
Kok
JH
.
Very preterm birth is associated with disabilities in multiple developmental domains
.
J Pediatr Psychol
.
2005
;
30
(
3
):
247
255
2
Bhutta
AT
,
Cleves
MA
,
Casey
PH
,
Cradock
MM
,
Anand
KJS
.
Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis
.
JAMA
.
2002
;
288
(
6
):
728
737
3
Anderson
P
,
Doyle
LW
;
Victorian Infant Collaborative Study Group
.
Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s
.
JAMA
.
2003
;
289
(
24
):
3264
3272
4
Taylor
HG
,
Minich
N
,
Bangert
B
,
Filipek
PA
,
Hack
M
.
Long-term neuropsychological outcomes of very low birth weight: associations with early risks for periventricular brain insults
.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc
.
2004
;
10
(
7
):
987
1004
5
Linsell
L
,
Malouf
R
,
Morris
J
,
Kurinczuk
JJ
,
Marlow
N
.
Prognostic factors for cerebral palsy and motor impairment in children born very preterm or very low birthweight: a systematic review
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2016
;
58
(
6
):
554
569
6
Einspieler
C
,
Prechtl
HF
,
Bos
AF
,
Ferrari
F
,
Cioni
G
.
Prechtl’s Method of the Qualitative Assessment of General Movements in Preterm, Term and Young Infants
.
London
:
MacKeith Press
;
2004
7
Einspieler
C
,
Bos
AF
,
Krieber-Tomantschger
M
, et al
.
Cerebral palsy: early markers of clinical phenotype and functional outcome
.
J Clin Med
.
2019
;
8
(
10
):
1616
8
Novak
I
,
Morgan
C
,
Adde
L
, et al
.
Early, accurate diagnosis and early intervention in cerebral palsy: advances in diagnosis and treatment
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2017
;
171
(
9
):
897
907
9
Spittle
AJ
,
Spencer-Smith
MM
,
Cheong
JLY
, et al
.
General movements in very preterm children and neurodevelopment at 2 and 4 years
.
Pediatrics
.
2013
;
132
(
2
):
e452
e458
10
Einspieler
C
,
Bos
AF
,
Libertus
ME
,
Marschik
PB
.
The general movement assessment helps us to identify preterm infants at risk for cognitive dysfunction
.
Front Psychol
.
2016
;
7
:
406
11
Bruggink
JLM
,
Einspieler
C
,
Butcher
PR
,
Stremmelaar
EF
,
Prechtl
HFR
,
Bos
AF
.
Quantitative aspects of the early motor repertoire in preterm infants: do they predict minor neurological dysfunction at school age?
Early Hum Dev
.
2009
;
85
(
1
):
25
36
12
Spittle
AJ
,
Anderson
PJ
,
Lee
KJ
, et al
.
Preventive care at home for very preterm infants improves infant and caregiver outcomes at 2 years
.
Pediatrics
.
2010
;
126
(
1
):
e171
e178
13
Spittle
AJ
,
Brown
NC
,
Doyle
LW
, et al
.
Quality of general movements is related to white matter pathology in very preterm infants
.
Pediatrics
.
2008
;
121
(
5
):
e1184
e1189
14
Spencer-Smith
MM
,
Spittle
AJ
,
Doyle
LW
, et al
.
Long-term benefits of home-based preventive care for preterm infants: a randomized trial
.
Pediatrics
.
2012
;
130
(
6
):
1094
1101
15
Roberts
G
,
Howard
K
,
Spittle
AJ
,
Brown
NC
,
Anderson
PJ
,
Doyle
LW
.
Rates of early intervention services in very preterm children with developmental disabilities at age 2 years
.
J Paediatr Child Health
.
2008
;
44
(
5
):
276
280
16
Einspieler
C
,
Prechtl
HFR
.
Prechtl’s assessment of general movements: a diagnostic tool for the functional assessment of the young nervous system
.
Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev
.
2005
;
11
(
1
):
61
67
17
Yuge
M
,
Marschik
PB
,
Nakajima
Y
, et al
.
Movements and postures of infants aged 3 to 5 months: to what extent is their optimality related to perinatal events and to the neurological outcome?
Early Hum Dev
.
2011
;
87
(
3
):
231
237
18
Spittle
AJ
,
Barton
S
,
Treyvaud
K
,
Molloy
CS
,
Doyle
LW
,
Anderson
PJ
.
School-Age Outcomes of Early Intervention for Preterm Infants and Their Parents: A Randomized Trial
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
6
):
e20161363
19
Elliot
CD
.
The Differential Abilities Scales
, 2nd ed.
New York, NY
:
Pearson Education Inc
;
2007
20
Manly
T
,
Robertson
I
,
Anderson
V
,
Nimmo-Smith
I
.
The Test of Everyday Attention for Children
.
Bury S Edmunds, England
:
Thames Valley Test Company Ltd
;
1999
21
Pickering
S
,
Gathercole
S
London, U.K.
:
The Psychological Corporation
;
2001
22
Shallice
T
.
Specific impairments of planning
.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
.
1982
;
298
(
1089
):
199
209
23
Henderson
S
,
Sugden
D
,
Barnett
A
.
Movement Assessment Battery for Children
, 2nd ed.
London, U.K.
:
The Psychological Corporation
;
2007
.
24
Anderson
PJ
,
Burnett
A
.
Assessing developmental delay in early childhood - concerns with the Bayley-III scales
.
Clin Neuropsychol
.
2017
;
31
(
2
):
371
381
25
Butcher
PR
,
van Braeckel
K
,
Bouma
A
,
Einspieler
C
,
Stremmelaar
EF
,
Bos
AF
.
The quality of preterm infants’ spontaneous movements: an early indicator of intelligence and behaviour at school age
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2009
;
50
(
8
):
920
930
26
Hitzert
MM
,
Roze
E
,
Van Braeckel
KNJA
,
Bos
AF
.
Motor development in 3-month-old healthy term-born infants is associated with cognitive and behavioural outcomes at early school age
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2014
;
56
(
9
):
869
876
27
Grunewaldt
KH
,
Fjørtoft
T
,
Bjuland
KJ
, et al
.
Follow-up at age 10 years in ELBW children - functional outcome, brain morphology and results from motor assessments in infancy
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2014
;
90
(
10
):
571
578
28
Bruggink
JLM
,
Einspieler
C
,
Butcher
PR
,
Van Braeckel
KNJA
,
Prechtl
HFR
,
Bos
AF
.
The quality of the early motor repertoire in preterm infants predicts minor neurologic dysfunction at school age
.
J Pediatr
.
2008
;
153
(
1
):
32
39
29
Fjørtoft
T
,
Grunewaldt
KH
,
Løhaugen
GCC
,
Mørkved
S
,
Skranes
J
,
Evensen
KAI
.
Assessment of motor behaviour in high-risk-infants at 3 months predicts motor and cognitive outcomes in 10 years old children
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2013
;
89
(
10
):
787
793
30
Chugani
HT
,
Phelps
ME
,
Mazziotta
JC
.
Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development
.
Ann Neurol
.
1987
;
22
(
4
):
487
497
31
Prechtl
HFR
.
State of the art of a new functional assessment of the young nervous system. An early predictor of cerebral palsy
.
Early Hum Dev
.
1997
;
50
(
1
):
1
11
32
Woodward
LJ
,
Anderson
PJ
,
Austin
NC
,
Howard
K
,
Inder
TE
.
Neonatal MRI to predict neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants
.
N Engl J Med
.
2006
;
355
(
7
):
685
694

Competing Interests

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: A.J. Spittle and A.F. Bos are certified tutors of the GM Trust. No other disclosures were reported.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Supplementary data