BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES

Children entering kindergarten ready to learn are more likely to thrive. Inequitable access to high-quality, early educational settings creates early educational disparities. TipsByText, a text-message–based program for caregivers of young children, improves literacy of children in preschool, but efficacy for families without access to early childhood education was unknown.

METHODS

We conducted a randomized controlled trial with caregivers of 3- and 4-year-olds in 2 public pediatric clinics. Intervention caregivers received TipsByText 3 times a week for 7 months. At pre- and postintervention, we measured child literacy using the Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Tool (PALS-PreK) and caregiver involvement using the Parent Child Interactivity Scale (PCI). We estimated effects on PALS-PreK and PCI using multivariable linear regression.

RESULTS

We enrolled 644 families, excluding 263 because of preschool participation. Compared with excluded children, those included in the study had parents with lower income and educational attainment and who were more likely to be Spanish speaking. Three-quarters of enrollees completed pre- and postintervention assessments. Postintervention PALS-PreK scores revealed an unadjusted treatment effect of 0.260 (P = .040); adjusting for preintervention score, child age, and caregiver language, treatment effect was 0.209 (P = .016), equating to ∼3 months of literacy gains. Effects were greater for firstborn children (0.282 vs 0.178), children in 2-parent families (0.262 vs 0.063), and 4-year-olds (0.436 vs 0.107). The overall effect on PCI was not significant (1.221, P = .124).

CONCLUSIONS

The health sector has unique access to difficult-to-reach young children. With this clinic-based texting intervention, we reached underresourced families and increased child literacy levels.

What’s Known on the Subject:

Not all children are ready to start kindergarten: low-income families have limited access to preschool and other supports that promote school readiness. Nearly all these families engage with the medical sector repeatedly in the first 5 years of life.

What This Study Adds:

Interventions that are deployed via the medical sector can reach the most underresourced families and reduce educational disparities. Using a literacy-based texting program, we effectively promoted literacy in a group of children with high likelihood of not being school-ready.

Early childhood education (ECE) and adult health are substantially linked15  because children entering kindergarten ready to learn are well positioned for academic success. Conversely, poor education is a key social driver of intergenerational poverty.6  Early childhood adversity, including poverty, can impede a child’s development and long-term educational outcomes.713  The effect of poverty on life outcomes is mediated through many mechanisms, including fewer books in the home,1416  lower maternal education levels,17  more single-parent households,17  limited access to ECE,1820  and numerous financial and informational barriers,7,21,22  all of which contribute to kindergarten readiness. National studies reveal less than one-half of low-income children are ready to start kindergarten.2327  Northern California mirrors the nation: in our county, 38% of low-income children score “ready” at school entry, and, in our public clinic, only 13% of low-income, immigrant children aged 5 years were “kinder-ready.”22,28 

Pediatrics has unique, near-universal, regular access to children aged <5 years29  and enjoys high levels of trust: families report that, outside their own immediate family, the pediatrician is the most trusted voice.3033  Increasingly, the American Academy of Pediatrics is issuing guidelines for pediatricians to screen for psychosocial risks and developmental delays34  and to use evidence-based interventions and programs to promote school readiness.35,36  Although health clinics provide books to families37  and initiatives to mitigate the developmental impacts of poverty,38  school readiness disparities persist.

The ubiquity of the electronic health record makes texting patients a novel, scalable way to reach large patient populations. Texting has empowered adult behavior change, from weight loss39  to smoking cessation40  to college enrollment for high school graduates.41,42  Many texting programs have emerged for parents, but only 1, TipsByText (TbT), formally known as Ready4K, has revealed positive effects on child literacy development and parent-reported behaviors for children enrolled in preschool43  and has expanded to many states. However, TbT has not been tested in the clinical setting.

Pediatricians have access to the hardest-to-reach children, enjoy high levels of trust, and now have the ability to text families. This transdisciplinary study tests the hypothesis that pediatric clinics can use TbT to improve literacy in young children detached from the ECE system. Secondarily, we sought to determine the feasibility of pediatricians connecting caregivers to an early childhood literacy intervention via texting.

We screened electronic charts of patients aged 3–4 years with appointments between October 2017 and August 2019 in 2 large, county-based clinics. Children were ineligible if they had developmental delay, if the family language was not English or Spanish, if there were more patients than the research assistant (RA) could get to, or if the child was deemed too ill. Eligible families were approached for enrollment, and those who consented completed a demographic survey assessing child age and birth order, household structure (single or 2-parent), primary caretaker’s age, role (mother, father), highest education level, race and ethnicity, preferred language, household income, preschool enrollment, and child’s insurance type. Children in preschool or transitional kindergarten were then excluded. Remaining families were randomly assigned through block randomization stratified by parent language and child baseline age.

We measured early child literacy with the Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Tool (PALS-PreK), which the University of Virginia developed and validated for a diverse child population.44  The PALS-PreK measures preschoolers’ knowledge of literacy fundamentals: name writing, alphabet knowledge, sound awareness, print and word awareness, rhyme awareness, and nursery rhyme awareness (Supplemental Table 5). PALS-PreK has revealed reliability, with strong internal consistency measured by Cronbach’s α (0.75–0.93) and similar Guttman split-half scores (0.71–0.94).44 

We calculated composite PALS-PreK scores for Spanish and English separately by summing individual items. Because the 2 PALS-PreK versions slightly differ in content, we standardized composite scores for pre- and postscores by language. Additionally, pre- and postintervention scores were standardized, allowing for comparison, creating the possibility of negative preintervention scores. As in previous research,45  we did not include letter sounds and lowercase letters questions in the composite score because children must score well on previous sections to be given these items, and most did not qualify.

Parent–child language exchanges were measured using the Parent Child Interactivity Scale (PCI), which uses parent self-assessment and has been validated in similar populations.43  The PCI uses a 4-point Likert scale for 5 items assessing reading, such as “Looked at pictures together in a book,” and 10 items assessing other literacy activities, such as “Practiced rhyming words.” (Supplemental Table 6) The PCI has revealed internal consistency in English (Cronbach’s α = 0.83) and Spanish (Cronbach’s α = 0.84).45  We calculated reading (PCI Read), other activities (PCI Activities), and a composite score (PCI Total).

The research team conducted weekly check-ins to monitor data collection. Improvement procedures included standardized protocols and monitoring of interrater reliability over 9 months (May 2018 to February 2019).46  We conducted interrater reliability for 17 PALS-PreK assessments (5% of assessments) across 4 independent raters (1 primary, 3 secondary). The interclass correlation coefficient ranged from 0.97 to 1.0 across raters with no change over time. Bilingual RAs administered the PALS Pre-K, and the vast majority of RA assessments were blinded. Unblinding occurred when families mentioned receiving texts or when an RA had to cover a different team role; sensitivity analyses indicated no impact on results.

Intervention caregivers received texts supporting literacy promotion 3 times a week for 7 months.43  The texts were designed by educational professionals as a 2-generation, caregiver empowerment model drawing on family strengths. Texts were structured to build on one another and delivered in small bits, delivering a light cognitive load to the caregiver. Each week, adults received 3 texts about an academic skill or set of skills: a “FACT” text designed to inform and motivate caregivers; a “TIP” text to minimize the cognitive, emotional, and time burdens of high-quality parenting by providing caregivers with specific activities building on existing family routines; and a “GROWTH” text to provide encouragement and reinforcement. (Supplemental Table 7) Each text began with “Doc Says” to convey the pediatrician’s support. The control group did not receive texts.

We used descriptive statistics to summarize baseline child, caregiver, and household characteristics. We implemented univariate analyses using χ2 tests and Fisher’s exact tests for categorical data and t tests for continuous data to determine if excluded and included participants differed. We similarly assessed differences between control and intervention groups to test whether randomization was effectively matched.

For PALS-PreK and PCI, we calculated summary statistics at baseline (preintervention) and at follow-up 7 months later (postintervention). We used multivariable regression to compare postintervention scores between control and intervention participants and calculated 95% confidence intervals for estimated effect sizes. For each outcome, we ran 3 models: model 1 was used to estimate the bivariate relationship between treatment and outcome (unadjusted), model 2 was adjusted for baseline score, and model 3 was adjusted for baseline score, age in months, and caregiver language. We further conducted subgroup analyses to assess the heterogeneity of effects, examining child birth order (firstborn, not firstborn), family structure (single-parent family, not single-parent family), caregiver language (English, Spanish), caregiver education (less than high school, high school, some college or more), and child baseline age (≥48 months, <48 months). Because of the skewed distribution of the standardized PALS-PreK score, asymptotic normality may not hold, given finite samples.47,48  Therefore, we calculated the standard errors of our model estimates using bootstrapping for valid inference.49  Statistical analysis were conducted by using R version 3.6.2 (R Foundation, Vienna, Austria).50  All statistical tests were 2-sided and performed at .05 significance level.

The Stanford University Human Subjects Review Board and the Santa Clara Valley Medical Center Research and Human Subjects Committee approved the study.

We screened medical records for 1968 upcoming clinic appointments, identifying 914 potential participants (Fig 1). The majority of ineligible appointments were excluded because of developmental delay (n = 554, 53%). Of the 914 eligible participants, 270 declined, most for the reason of “not enough time on appointment date” (n = 153, 57%). Of eligible participants, 644 completed the demographic survey, after which we excluded an additional 263 (41%) because of preschool or transitional kindergarten enrollment. We randomly assigned 381 participants to the intervention or control group, and 10 caregivers opted out of receiving texts. At the postintervention time, 275 (74%) completed the postintervention assessment, 29 (8%) declined to participate, and 67 (18%) could not be reached. Those who did and did not participate at postintervention (n = 106 lost to follow-up) were similar in terms of sibling order, total children, caregiver age and race and ethnicity, household income, and insurance type (Supplemental Table 8). The groups differed in some respects: those lost to follow-up had slightly older children (43 vs 45 months) with more caregivers who were fathers, spoke English, and attended college. We found no evidence of difference in attrition between intervention and control groups in the proportion leaving or in the characteristics of those who left.

FIGURE 1

TbT randomized trial subject enrollment, October 2017–July 2019.

FIGURE 1

TbT randomized trial subject enrollment, October 2017–July 2019.

Close modal

Included participants and those ineligible largely because of preschool enrollment were similar in family structure and caregiver age but differed across several sociodemographic characteristics (Table 1). Eligible participants came from families with lower income and lower educational attainment (65% vs 83% with a high school education or greater). They were slightly younger and more likely to have a Spanish-speaking caregiver (46% vs 29%).

TABLE 1

Baseline Sociodemographic Characteristics of Ineligible Due to Preschool Enrollment and Included Study Participants

Included
CharacteristicsOverallIneligibleIncludedPaControlInterventionPb
Total 644 263 381  204 177  
Child age, mean (SD), mo 46 (7.08) 48 (6.63) 44 (6.85) <.001 44 (6.90) 44 (6.81) .71 
Child age ≥48 mo, n (%) 263 (46) 127 (66) 136 (36) <.001 76 (37) 60 (34) .59 
Child birth order, mean (SD) 2 (1.16) 2 (1.04) 2 (1.23) .05 2 (1.26) 2 (1.19) .70 
Family child number, mean (SD) 2 (1.21) 2 (1.10) 2 (1.28) .13 2 (1.31) 2 (1.25) .36 
Family structure, n (%)        
 2-parent family 455 (73) 193 (74) 262 (71) .61 130 (67) 132 (76) .12 
 Single-parent family 153 (24) 61 (24) 92 (25) — 53 (27) 39 (22) — 
 Other 9 (1) 2 (0.8) 7 (2) — 5 (3) 2 (1) — 
 Prefer not to answer 11 (2) 4 (2) 7 (2) — 6 (3) 1 (0.6) — 
Caregiver age, mean (SD), y 33 (6.69) 33 (6.01) 33 (7.09) .71 33 (7.34) 32 (6.82) .68 
Caregiver role, n (%)    .05   .42 
 Father 76 (12) 41 (16) 35 (9) — 20 (10) 15 (9) — 
 Mother 556 (87) 218 (83) 338 (89) — 177 (88) 161 (91) — 
 Other 8 (1) 3 (1) 5 (1) — 4 (2) 1 (0.6) — 
Caregiver education, n (%)    <.001   .55 
 Less than high school 168 (27) 42 (17) 126 (35) — 71 (37) 55 (32) — 
 High school 180 (29) 66 (26) 114 (31) — 61 (32) 53 (31) — 
 Some college 180 (29) 92 (37) 88 (24) — 41 (21) 47 (28) — 
 College+ 88 (14) 52 (21) 36 (10) — 20 (10) 16 (10) — 
Caregiver race and ethnicity, n (%)    <.001   .71 
 Hispanic 415 (66) 145 (55) 270 (73) — 149 (75) 121 (70) — 
 White 55 (9) 33 (13) 22 (6) — 10 (5) 12 (7) — 
 Black or African American 40 (6) 23 (9) 17 (5) — 10 (5) 7 (4) — 
 Asian American 61 (10) 37 (14) 24 (7) — 12 (6) 12 (7) — 
 Multiple races or ethnicities 39 (6) 17 (7) 22 (6) — 11 (6) 11 (6) — 
 Other 23 (4) 7 (3) 16 (4) — 6 (3) 10 (6) — 
Caregiver language Spanish, n (%) 252 (40) 76 (29) 176 (46) <.001 97 (48) 79 (45) .64 
Household income, n (%)    .005   .77 
 <$12 000 65 (10) 20 (8) 45 (12) — 25 (13) 20 (11) — 
 $12 001–$24 000 172 (27) 53 (21) 119 (32) — 57 (29) 62 (35) — 
 $24 001–$36 000 137 (22) 61 (24) 76 (20) — 43 (22) 33 (19) — 
 $36 001–$48 000 74 (12) 35 (14) 39 (10) — 20 (10) 19 (11) — 
 >$48 000 90 (14) 46 (18) 44 (12) — 24 (12) 20 (11) — 
 Decline to respond 94 (15) 43 (17) 51 (14) — 30 (15) 21 (12) — 
Insurance type, n (%)       .65 
 Medicaid 336 (89) — 336 (89) — 176 (87) 160 (90) — 
 Commercial 37 (10) — 37 (10) — 22 (11) 15 (9) — 
 Other (private) 6 (1) — 6 (1) — 4 (2) 2 (1) — 
Included
CharacteristicsOverallIneligibleIncludedPaControlInterventionPb
Total 644 263 381  204 177  
Child age, mean (SD), mo 46 (7.08) 48 (6.63) 44 (6.85) <.001 44 (6.90) 44 (6.81) .71 
Child age ≥48 mo, n (%) 263 (46) 127 (66) 136 (36) <.001 76 (37) 60 (34) .59 
Child birth order, mean (SD) 2 (1.16) 2 (1.04) 2 (1.23) .05 2 (1.26) 2 (1.19) .70 
Family child number, mean (SD) 2 (1.21) 2 (1.10) 2 (1.28) .13 2 (1.31) 2 (1.25) .36 
Family structure, n (%)        
 2-parent family 455 (73) 193 (74) 262 (71) .61 130 (67) 132 (76) .12 
 Single-parent family 153 (24) 61 (24) 92 (25) — 53 (27) 39 (22) — 
 Other 9 (1) 2 (0.8) 7 (2) — 5 (3) 2 (1) — 
 Prefer not to answer 11 (2) 4 (2) 7 (2) — 6 (3) 1 (0.6) — 
Caregiver age, mean (SD), y 33 (6.69) 33 (6.01) 33 (7.09) .71 33 (7.34) 32 (6.82) .68 
Caregiver role, n (%)    .05   .42 
 Father 76 (12) 41 (16) 35 (9) — 20 (10) 15 (9) — 
 Mother 556 (87) 218 (83) 338 (89) — 177 (88) 161 (91) — 
 Other 8 (1) 3 (1) 5 (1) — 4 (2) 1 (0.6) — 
Caregiver education, n (%)    <.001   .55 
 Less than high school 168 (27) 42 (17) 126 (35) — 71 (37) 55 (32) — 
 High school 180 (29) 66 (26) 114 (31) — 61 (32) 53 (31) — 
 Some college 180 (29) 92 (37) 88 (24) — 41 (21) 47 (28) — 
 College+ 88 (14) 52 (21) 36 (10) — 20 (10) 16 (10) — 
Caregiver race and ethnicity, n (%)    <.001   .71 
 Hispanic 415 (66) 145 (55) 270 (73) — 149 (75) 121 (70) — 
 White 55 (9) 33 (13) 22 (6) — 10 (5) 12 (7) — 
 Black or African American 40 (6) 23 (9) 17 (5) — 10 (5) 7 (4) — 
 Asian American 61 (10) 37 (14) 24 (7) — 12 (6) 12 (7) — 
 Multiple races or ethnicities 39 (6) 17 (7) 22 (6) — 11 (6) 11 (6) — 
 Other 23 (4) 7 (3) 16 (4) — 6 (3) 10 (6) — 
Caregiver language Spanish, n (%) 252 (40) 76 (29) 176 (46) <.001 97 (48) 79 (45) .64 
Household income, n (%)    .005   .77 
 <$12 000 65 (10) 20 (8) 45 (12) — 25 (13) 20 (11) — 
 $12 001–$24 000 172 (27) 53 (21) 119 (32) — 57 (29) 62 (35) — 
 $24 001–$36 000 137 (22) 61 (24) 76 (20) — 43 (22) 33 (19) — 
 $36 001–$48 000 74 (12) 35 (14) 39 (10) — 20 (10) 19 (11) — 
 >$48 000 90 (14) 46 (18) 44 (12) — 24 (12) 20 (11) — 
 Decline to respond 94 (15) 43 (17) 51 (14) — 30 (15) 21 (12) — 
Insurance type, n (%)       .65 
 Medicaid 336 (89) — 336 (89) — 176 (87) 160 (90) — 
 Commercial 37 (10) — 37 (10) — 22 (11) 15 (9) — 
 Other (private) 6 (1) — 6 (1) — 4 (2) 2 (1) — 

Percentages are rounded to whole numbers. —, not applicable.

a

Test between ineligible and included groups.

b

Test between control and intervention group.

The intervention and control groups had similar demographic backgrounds, family composition, and socioeconomic status (Table 1). Children were an average of 44 months old, and 89% attended clinic with their mothers (average age 33 years). Participants were predominantly Hispanic (73%) with a minority identifying as non-Hispanic white (6%), Black or African American (5%), Asian American (7%), multiracial or ethnic (6%), or other (4%). Almost one-half of participants indicated Spanish was their preferred language. Participants came largely from low-income, 2-parent families (64% with a family income <$36 001; 71% from a 2-parent family) with Medicaid insurance (88.7%). Baseline standardized PALS-PreK scores were similar for control and intervention groups on average (−0.38 [SD = 0.76] vs −0.32 [SD = 0.83]), as were total PCI scores (24.40 [SD = 9.68] vs 24.08 [SD = 9.97]). Postintervention, standardized PALS-PreK scores were, on average, 0.36 (SD = 0.98) for the control group compared with 0.62 (SD = 1.08) for the intervention group. Total PCI scores were 25.96 (SD = 10.0) for the control, versus 28.34 (SD = 9.15) for the intervention group.

TbT’s impact on the children’s literacy development is revealed in the intervention’s estimated effect on follow-up PALS Pre-K scores (Table 2). Model 1 gives the unadjusted overall intervention effect of 0.260 (95% CI: 0.121 to 0.507, P = .040). Model 2 is adjusted for the baseline PALS-PreK score, with estimated effect of 0.202 (CI: 0.031 to 0.372, P = .021). Model 3 is adjusted for baseline PALS-PreK score, baseline child age, and caregiver language, with an estimated effect size of 0.209 (CI: 0.038 to 0.378, P = .016). Including the additional controls had little effect on the point estimates but reduced the standard errors and, as a result, made the estimates more precise. In Model 3, although caregiver language being Spanish was not associated with postintervention PALS-PreK score (−0.053, CI: −0.219 to −0.113, P = .532), child age did have an independent contribution (0.029, CI: 0.014 to 0.043, P < .001), as did baseline PALS-PreK score (0.813, CI: 0.702 to 0.923, P < .001).

TABLE 2

Estimated Effect and 95% CIs of TbT Intervention on Child Follow-up PALS-PreK Score

Model 1Model 2Model 3
TbT intervention 0.260 0.202 0.209 
 CI 0.012 to 0.507 0.031 to 0.372 0.039 to 0.378 
P .040 .021 .016 
Baseline PALS-PreK score — 0.921 0.813 
 CI — 0.825 to 0.372 0.702 to 0.923 
P — <0.001 <0.001 
Baseline child age, mo — — 0.029 
 CI — — 0.014 to 0.043 
P — — <.001 
Assessment language, Spanish — — −0.053 
 CI — — −0.219 to 0.113 
P — — .532 
Model 1Model 2Model 3
TbT intervention 0.260 0.202 0.209 
 CI 0.012 to 0.507 0.031 to 0.372 0.039 to 0.378 
P .040 .021 .016 
Baseline PALS-PreK score — 0.921 0.813 
 CI — 0.825 to 0.372 0.702 to 0.923 
P — <0.001 <0.001 
Baseline child age, mo — — 0.029 
 CI — — 0.014 to 0.043 
P — — <.001 
Assessment language, Spanish — — −0.053 
 CI — — −0.219 to 0.113 
P — — .532 

Model 1: overall treatment effect (n = 275). Model 2: adjusts for baseline PALS-PreK score (n = 275). Model 3: adjusts for baseline PALS-PreK score, baseline child age, and assessment language (n = 275). —, not applicable.

To provide real-world context for TbT effects, we compared them to typical learning rates. When we estimate the relationship between child age and postintervention PALS-PreK scores for those in the control group, the estimate is 0.066 (CI: 0.043 to 0.089, P < .001). Thus, in a normal routine, children in our sample would have gained 0.066 points on the PALS-PreK assessment per month. Those in the intervention group gained an additional 0.202 to 0.260 depending on the model, which translates to 3 months of learning. Thus, the intervention effect on literacy scores equates to 3 months of literacy gains for children.

We examined the heterogeneity in the TbT effects on postintervention PALS-PreK score by calculating the estimated effect for subgroups.(Table 3) The intervention effect was evident for firstborn children (0.282, CI: 0.021 to 0.542, P = .034), 2-parent families (0.262, CI: 0.063 to 0.461, P = .010), English-speaking caregivers (0.238, CI: 0.003 to 0.473, P = .047), and children aged ≥48 months (0.436, CI: 0.116 to 0.755, P = .008). We found no difference by caregiver level of education.

TABLE 3

Heterogeneity in TbT Intervention Effects on Literacy

nEstimated Effect95% CIP
Full sample 275 0.209 0.039 to 0.378 .016 
Child birth order     
 Firstborn 95 0.282 0.021 to 0.542 .034 
 Not firstborn 164 0.178 −0.053 to 0.408 .131 
Family structure     
 Single-parent family 60 0.063 −0.257 to 0.383 .700 
 Not single-parent family 199 0.262 0.063 to 0.461 .010 
Caregiver language     
 English 127 0.238 0.003 to 0.473 .047 
 Spanish 148 0.188 −0.048 to 0.423 .119 
Caregiver education     
 Less than high school 95 0.184 −0.113 to 0.480 .225 
 High school 90 0.186 −0.108 to 0.479 .215 
 Some college or more 77 0.221 −0.083 to 0.525 .154 
Child baseline age, category     
 ≥48 mo 86 0.436 0.116 to 0.755 .008 
 <48 mo 189 0.107 −0.088 to 0.302 .283 
nEstimated Effect95% CIP
Full sample 275 0.209 0.039 to 0.378 .016 
Child birth order     
 Firstborn 95 0.282 0.021 to 0.542 .034 
 Not firstborn 164 0.178 −0.053 to 0.408 .131 
Family structure     
 Single-parent family 60 0.063 −0.257 to 0.383 .700 
 Not single-parent family 199 0.262 0.063 to 0.461 .010 
Caregiver language     
 English 127 0.238 0.003 to 0.473 .047 
 Spanish 148 0.188 −0.048 to 0.423 .119 
Caregiver education     
 Less than high school 95 0.184 −0.113 to 0.480 .225 
 High school 90 0.186 −0.108 to 0.479 .215 
 Some college or more 77 0.221 −0.083 to 0.525 .154 
Child baseline age, category     
 ≥48 mo 86 0.436 0.116 to 0.755 .008 
 <48 mo 189 0.107 −0.088 to 0.302 .283 

Each row represents an individual model estimating the effect of the intervention on PALS-PreK standardized score, stratified by each of the groups listed above. All models are adjusted for baseline PALS-PreK score, child baseline age, and caregiver language.

The PCI scale was used to assess caregivers’ self-reported literacy interactions with their children (Table 4). The reading and total PCI scores did not reveal a significant change across unadjusted or adjusted analyses. The unadjusted intervention effect on postintervention PCI literacy activities was significant (1.96, CI: 0.167 to 3.76, P = .033); however, the intervention effect was not statistically significant when adjusted in Model 2 (1.25, CI: −0.329 to 2.83, P = .122) or Model 3 (1.22, CI: −0.328 to 2.77, P = .124).

TABLE 4

Estimated Effect and 95% CIs of TbT Intervention on Follow-up PCI Literacy Activities And Reading; Summaries of Pre- and Postintervention PCI Scores Stratified by Group

Model 1Model 2Model 3
PCI activities 1.96 1.25 1.22 
 CI 0.167 to 3.70 −0.329 to 2.83 −0.328 to 2.77 
P .033 .122 .124 
PCI Reading 0.510 0.248 0.242 
 CI −0.542 to 1.60 −0.822 to 1.32 −0.839 to 1.32 
P .346 .650 0.661 
Preintervention Overall Control Intervention 
 PCI total scorea 24.26 (9.80) 24.40 (9.68) 24.08 (9.97) 
 PCI reading 8.09 (4.03) 8.21 (3.98) 7.94 (4.10) 
 PCI activities 15.40 (7.10) 15.37 (7.09) 15.45 (7.14) 
Postintervention Overall Control Intervention 
 PCI total score 27.02 (9.68) 25.96 (10.00) 28.34 (9.15) 
 PCI reading 8.58 (3.99) 8.36 (4.17) 8.87 (3.75) 
 PCI activities 16.92 (7.40) 16.05 (7.41) 18.02 (7.26) 
Model 1Model 2Model 3
PCI activities 1.96 1.25 1.22 
 CI 0.167 to 3.70 −0.329 to 2.83 −0.328 to 2.77 
P .033 .122 .124 
PCI Reading 0.510 0.248 0.242 
 CI −0.542 to 1.60 −0.822 to 1.32 −0.839 to 1.32 
P .346 .650 0.661 
Preintervention Overall Control Intervention 
 PCI total scorea 24.26 (9.80) 24.40 (9.68) 24.08 (9.97) 
 PCI reading 8.09 (4.03) 8.21 (3.98) 7.94 (4.10) 
 PCI activities 15.40 (7.10) 15.37 (7.09) 15.45 (7.14) 
Postintervention Overall Control Intervention 
 PCI total score 27.02 (9.68) 25.96 (10.00) 28.34 (9.15) 
 PCI reading 8.58 (3.99) 8.36 (4.17) 8.87 (3.75) 
 PCI activities 16.92 (7.40) 16.05 (7.41) 18.02 (7.26) 

Model 1: overall intervention effect (n = 261 for activities, n = 219 for reading). Model 2: adjusts for baseline PCI score (n = 251, n = 188). Model 3: adjusts for baseline PCI score, baseline child age, and caregiver language (n = 251, n = 188).

a

Mean and SD are shown for each score.

Profound educational disparities are evident across socioeconomic groups at kindergarten entry. With this literacy-promoting texting program, we leveraged a pediatric care setting and increased literacy levels for a group of underresourced children by an average of 3 months. The effects varied across subgroups: firstborn and older children revealed more gains. These differential effects within our sample are not intended to guide clinical application. These findings are shared so that they may inform the design of future research into the effects of this program and parenting programs more generally. Overall, this is not one size fits all intervention.

Young children in the United States have greater access to health care than to early education because health care for young children is near universal. The Early and Periodic Screening, Diagnostic and Treatment benefit of Medicaid ensures all children (aged <18–21, state depending) are entitled to routine preventive services and medical treatment. Overall 49% of children aged 3–4 years are enrolled in preschool, with lower enrollment rates for those with parents who have lower educational levels,51  nearly 99% of children were hospital born,52  and, at 5 years of age, 94% have received required immunizations for school entry.53  As we have revealed, this cohort of low-income, publicly insured children represents a heterogeneous population. In our sample, those not in preschool had caregivers with significantly lower education and income and were more likely to identify as Hispanic and to speak Spanish at home. Educational interventions deployed from clinic can reach a distinct, more underresourced subset of families; this finding adds to our knowledge about the unique contribution the health sector can make toward addressing school readiness. More evidence-based, scalable programs are needed for younger and older children, as well as for those enrolled in preschool, to narrow ECE inequities.

TbT has the potential to move pediatrics to a higher level of impact. Past experience that scaled texting programs for parents through the education sector is informative. Within a few years after completing initial studies among preschool families in the San Francisco Unified School District, this program scaled quickly. The original research team has sent >40 million parenting texts to families in public preschools through the school districts in Florida, Georgia, New Jersey, Pennsylvania, Texas, and Wisconsin, among others. Large scale and rapid uptake were facilitated by the transformative ubiquity of personal cell phones, texting as a well-received modality of parent communication, and the intervention being inexpensive and scalable by design. However, although school districts have been instrumental in reaching families with children in preschool, they do not reach families with younger children or those not attending preschool. Health clinics can provide access to these families, but evidence of clear benefits for families and children are needed to justify moving to scale.

We used the PCI to assess program effects on parent self-reported behaviors and to better understand literacy improvement. The study was designed to measure the primary outcome of child literacy gains using PALS, rendering it underpowered to detect differences measured by PCI parent self-report. This choice was acceptable to investigators because early studies in preschools using teacher and parent reports detected positive impact on parenting behavior, as measured by PCI and teacher surveys, establishing the TbT causal mechanism.45  Of note, at baseline, caregivers reported high reading levels, indicating that messaging to read to children may be reaching saturation, a fact to celebrate. However, caregivers who have heard they should be reading to their children may overreport reading, suggesting parent reports might not be accurate, given strong societal pressures. Tools that measure language directly by recording and counting number of words spoken54  may better assess parental behavior.

TbT’s structure and scaffolding are based on extensive theoretical models that address caregiver challenges of low-resourced communities, including economic stress that requires adults to focus on short-term over long-term outcomes. For instance, a focus on having food for the week would displace a caregiver’s attention on longer term educational outcomes. TbT is designed to address these barriers, to break down educational processes into bite-size components that come via small doses every other day over a long period of time. As other nontexting clinic-based interventions emerge, such as individualized parent coaching,55,56  library referrals,57  electronic preschool enrollment,58  or community referral coordinators,59  sustained attention to caregiver needs and their day-to-day barriers will likely be important to overcoming the persistent differences in school readiness across groups.

The medical sector is increasingly motivated to address issues of poverty and its antecedent factors.36,60  New advances in ECE are needed to address social-emotional learning, numeracy, literacy, and ways to support the development of newborn-to-3-year-olds. It requires humility to acknowledge that medical training does not prepare us to build effective ECE interventions. The role of convergence science,61  founded on trusting, transdisciplinary partnerships with schools of education, holds promise for transformative advances.

This study has a number of limitations. Northern California is racially diverse, with a significant Hispanic population, limiting generalizability. It remains to be seen whether rural families or urban, African American families would respond similarly, although early findings show promise.45  Demographic data could not be gathered from those declining participation, so how they vary from participants is unknown. The PCI tool relied on parent self-report, which may have been impacted by social desirability bias and a ceiling effect, particularly for reading. Fidelity concerns are always warranted, but treatment fidelity is unusually strong in a text-message program. The caregivers we texted may have changed caregiving responsibilities or allowed others to use their phone. A final limitation is that the study is based on 1 measure of child development, and TbT can affect many aspects of child development beyond what PALS-PreK assesses.

This study revealed that a texting intervention sent from a clinical setting could reach a underresourced population of families, resulting in 3 months of literacy gains for young children. Further studies of clinic-based texting interventions exploring the causal factors linked to child outcomes are warranted. Leveraging the health sector’s unparalleled access to young children presents an opportunity to narrow educational disparities.

Drs Chamberlain and Loeb conceptualized and designed the study, contributed to the analytic plan, drafted the initial manuscript, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Bruce conceptualized and designed the study, drafted the initial manuscript, reviewed and revised the manuscript, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Steinberg, Ms De La Cruz, and Mx Bruguera designed the data collection instruments, collected data, summarized data, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Ms Ordaz and Ms Connelly collected data and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Peterson contributed to the study’s conception and design and reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Huffman conducted the initial analyses and contributed to the interpretation of data and reviewed and revised the manuscript for important intellectual content; Drs Gardner and He contributed to the analytic plan, conducted the analyses, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

This study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov, #NCT04752943, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT04752943. Deidentified data will not be made available.

Ms De La Cruz is currently affiliated with University of California Berkeley, Berkeley California; D. Steinberg is currently affiliated with Northwestern Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, Chicago, Illinois; Mx Bruguera is currently affiliated with School of Medicine, University of California, Davis, California; Dr Loeb is currently affiliated with Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island.

FUNDING: Supported by a grant from the Stupski Foundation to Drs Chamberlain and Bruce. The funder had no role in the design and conduct of the study.

COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found online at www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2021-052326.

ECE

early childhood education

PALS-PreK

Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening Tool

PCI

Parent Child Interactivity Scale

RA

research assistant

TbT

TipsByText

1
Campbell
F
,
Conti
G
,
Heckman
JJ
, et al
.
Early childhood investments substantially boost adult health
.
Science
.
2014
;
343
(
6178
):
1478
1485
2
Yoshikawa
H
.
Long-term effects of early childhood programs on social outcomes and delinquency
.
Future Child
.
1995
;
5
(
3
):
51
75
3
Campbell
FA
,
Ramey
CT
,
Pungello
E
,
Sparling
J
,
Miller-Johnson
S
.
Early childhood education: young adult outcomes from the Abecedarian Project
.
Appl Dev Sci
.
2002
;
6
(
1
):
42
57
4
Barnett
WS
.
Lives in the Balance: Age-27 Benefit-Cost Analysis of the High/Scope Perry Preschool Program. Monographs of the High/Scope Educational Research Foundation, Number Eleven
.
Washington, DC
:
ERIC
;
1996
5
Heckman
JJ
,
Moon
SH
,
Pinto
R
,
Savelyev
PA
,
Yavitz
A
.
The rate of return to the high/scope Perry Preschool Program
.
J Public Econ
.
2010
;
94
(
1–2
):
114
128
6
Medlin
C
,
Harper
M
.
Investing in health for economic growth and poverty reduction: new perspectives and opportunities
.
Med Confl Surviv
.
2003
;
19
(
2
):
165
174
7
Bradley
RH
,
Convyn
RF
,
Burchinal
M
,
McAdoo
HP
,
Coll
CG
.
The home environments of children in the United States part II: relations with behavioral development through age thirteen
.
Child Dev
.
2001
;
72
(
6
):
1868
1886
8
Johnson
SB
,
Riis
JL
,
Noble
KG
.
State of the art review: poverty and the developing brain
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
137
(
4
):
e20153075
9
Council on Community Pediatrics
.
Poverty and child health in the United States
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
137
(
4
):
e20160339
10
Shonkoff
JP
,
Garner
AS
;
Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health; Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care; Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics
.
The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress
.
Pediatrics
.
2012
;
129
(
1
):
e232
e246
11
Giovanelli
A
,
Reynolds
AJ
,
Mondi
CF
,
Ou
SR
.
Adverse childhood experiences and adult well-being in a low-income, urban cohort
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
137
(
4
):
e20154016
12
Petterson
SM
,
Albers
AB
.
Effects of poverty and maternal depression on early child development
.
Child Dev
.
2001
;
72
(
6
):
1794
1813
13
McLoyd
VC
.
Socioeconomic disadvantage and child development
.
Am Psychol
.
1998
;
53
(
2
):
185
204
14
Hart
B
,
Risley
TR
.
Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children
.
Baltimore, MD
:
Paul H Brookes Publishing
;
1995
15
Yoshikawa
H
,
Weiland
C
,
Brooks-Gunn
J
, et al
.
Investing in Our Future: The Evidence Base on Preschool Education
.
Washington, DC
:
Society for Research in Child Development
;
2013
16
Golinkoff
RM
,
Hoff
E
,
Rowe
ML
,
Tamis-LeMonda
CS
,
Hirsh-Pasek
K
.
Language matters: denying the existence of the 30-million-word gap has serious consequences
.
Child Dev
.
2019
;
90
(
3
):
985
992
17
Hagan
JFSJ
,
Duncan
PM
, eds.
Bright Futures: Guidelines for Health Supervision of Infants, Children, and Adolescents
, 4th ed.
Elk Grove Village, IL
:
American Academy of Pediatrics
;
2017
18
National Institute for Early Education Research
.
State of Preschool 2017: State Preschool Yearbook
.
New Brunswick, NJ
:
Rutgers Graduate School of Education
;
2017
19
Gormley
WT
 Jr
,
Phillips
DA
,
Newmark
K
,
Welti
K
,
Adelstein
S
.
Social-emotional effects of early childhood education programs in Tulsa
.
Child Dev
.
2011
;
82
(
6
):
2095
2109
20
Magnuson
KA
,
Meyers
MK
,
Ruhm
CJ
,
Waldfogel
J
.
Inequality in preschool education and school readiness
.
Am Educ Res J
.
2004
;
41
(
1
):
115
157
21
Lareau
A
.
Unequal childhoods: Class, Race, and Family Life
.
Berkeley, CA
:
University of California Press
;
2011
22
Peterson
J
,
Bruce
J
,
Patel
N
,
Chamberlain
LJ
.
Parental attitudes, behaviors, and barriers to school readiness among parents of low-income Latino children
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2018
;
15
(
2
):
188
23
Lee
VE
,
Burkham
D
.
Inequality at the Starting gate: Social Background Differences in Achievement as Children Begin Kindergarten
.
Washington, DC
:
Economic Policy Institute
;
2002
24
Bassok
D
,
Fitzpatrick
M
,
Greenberg
E
,
Loeb
S
.
Within- and between-sector quality differences in early childhood education and care
.
Child Dev
.
2016
;
87
(
5
):
1627
1645
25
Reardon
SF
. In:
Duncan
GJ
,
Murnane
RJ
, eds.
Whither Opportunity: Rising Inequality, Schools, and Children’s Life Chances
.
New York, NYk
:
Russell Sage Foundation
;
2011
:
91
116
26
Bassok
D
,
Finch
JE
,
Lee
R
,
Reardon
SF
,
Waldfogel
J
.
Socioeconomic gaps in early childhood experiences: 1998 to 2010
.
AERA Open
.
2016
;
2
:
3
27
Garcia
E
,
Weiss
E
.
Education Inequalities at the School Starting Gate: Gaps, Trends, and Strategies to Address Them
.
Washington, DC
:
Economic Policy Institute
;
2017
28
Gomez
C
,
Cannon
JS
,
Whitaker
L
,
Karoly
LA
.
Big Lift Participation and School Entry Indicators: Findings for the 2016-2017 Kindergarten Class
.
Santa Monica, CA
:
RAND Corporation
;
2017
29
Child and Adolescent Health Measurement Initiative
.
Health Care Access and Quality 2017-18
. In:
Data Resource Center for Child and Adolescent Health
.
Washington, DC
:
US Department of Health and Human Services
;
2018
30
Center for Education and Human Services
.
School Readiness Assessment: 2012
.
Menlo Park, CA
:
SRI International
;
2012
31
Brown
CM
,
Girio-Herrera
EL
,
Sherman
SN
,
Kahn
RS
,
Copeland
KA
.
Low-income parents’ perceptions of pediatrician advice on early childhood education
.
J Community Health
.
2013
;
38
(
1
):
195
204
32
Steinberg
JR
,
Bruce
JS
,
Marin-Nevarez
P
,
Phan
K
,
Merrell
SB
,
Chamberlain
LJ
.
Early childhood learning and the pediatrician: a qualitative study among diverse, low-income caregivers
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2018
;
39
(
5
):
376
386
33
Center for Early Learning
.
Connecting Home and the Pediatrician’s Office to Achieve School Readiness in Silicon Valley
.
Mountain View, CA
:
Silicon Valley Community Foundation
;
2019
34
Lipkin
PH
,
Macias
MM
;
Council on Children With Disabilities, Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics
.
Promoting optimal development: identifying infants and young children with developmental disorders through developmental surveillance and screening
.
Pediatrics
.
2020
;
145
(
1
):
e20193449
35
Williams
PG
,
Lerner
MA
;
Council on Early Childhood; Council on School Health
.
School readiness
.
Pediatrics
.
2019
;
144
(
2
):
e20191766
36
American Academy of Pediatrics
.
AAP Agenda for Children Strategic Plan: Poverty and Child Health
.
2014. Elk Grove Village, IL: AAP; 2014. Available at: https://www.aap.org/en-us/advocacy-and-policy/aap-health-initiatives/poverty/Pages/About-Us.aspx. Accessed March 3, 2018
37
Zuckerman
B
,
Khandekar
A
.
Reach Out and Read: evidence based approach to promoting early child development
.
Curr Opin Pediatr
.
2010
;
22
(
4
):
539
544
38
Cates
CB
,
Weisleder
A
,
Mendelsohn
AL
.
Mitigating the effects of family poverty on early child development through parenting interventions in primary care
.
Acad Pediatr
.
2016
;
16
(
3 Suppl
):
S112
S120
39
Patrick
K
,
Raab
F
,
Adams
MA
, et al
.
A text message-based intervention for weight loss: randomized controlled trial
.
J Med Internet Res
.
2009
;
11
(
1
):
e1
40
Rodgers
A
,
Corbett
T
,
Bramley
D
, et al
.
Do u smoke after txt? Results of a randomised trial of smoking cessation using mobile phone text messaging
.
Tob Control
.
2005
;
14
(
4
):
255
261
41
Marcolino
MS
,
Oliveira
JAQ
,
D’Agostino
M
,
Ribeiro
AL
,
Alkmim
MBM
,
Novillo-Ortiz
D
.
The impact of mHealth interventions: systematic review of systematic reviews
.
JMIR Mhealth Uhealth
.
2018
;
6
(
1
):
e23
42
Castleman
BL
,
Page
LC
.
Summer nudging: Can personalized text messages and peer mentor outreach increase college going among low-income high school graduates?
J Econ Behav Organ
.
2015
;
115
:
144
160
43
Doss
C
,
Fahle
EM
,
Loeb
S
,
York
BN
.
More than just a nudge: supporting kindergarten parents with differentiated and personalized text messages
.
J Hum Resour
.
2019
;
54
(
3
):
567
603
44
Invernizzi
M
,
Sullivan
A
,
Meier
J
,
Swank
L
.
Phonological Awareness Literacy Screening–PreK
.
Charlottesville, VA
:
University of Virginia
;
2004
45
York
BN
,
Loeb
S
,
Doss
C
.
One step at a time: the effects of an early literacy text-messaging program for parents of preschoolers
.
J Hum Resour
.
2019
;
54
(
3
):
537
566
46
Liddy
C
,
Wiens
M
,
Hogg
W
.
Methods to achieve high interrater reliability in data collection from primary care medical records
.
Ann Fam Med
.
2011
;
9
(
1
):
57
62
47
Rascati
KL
,
Smith
MJ
,
Neilands
T
.
Dealing with skewed data: an example using asthma-related costs of medicaid clients
.
Clin Ther
.
2001
;
23
(
3
):
481
498
48
Visalakshi
J
,
Jeyaseelan
L
.
Confidence interval for skewed distribution in outcome of change or difference between methods
.
Clin Epidemiol Glob Health
.
2014
;
2
(
3
):
117
120
49
Efron
B
,
Tibshirani
RJ
.
An Introduction to the Bootstrap
.
Boca Raton, FL
:
CRC press
;
1994
50
R Core Team
.
R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing
.
Vienna, Austria
:
R Foundation for Statistical Computing
;
2013
51
Institute of Education Sciences
.
The Condition of Education: Enrollment Rates of Young Children
.
Washington DC
:
National Center for Educational Statistics
.
2021
.
Available at: https://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator/cfa. Accessed August 16, 2021
52
MacDorman
M
,
Mathews
MS
,
Declercq
E
.
Trends in Out-of-Hospital Births in the United States, 1990–2012
.
NCHS data brief, no 144
.
Hyattsville, Maryland
:
National Center for Health Statistics
;
2014
.
53
Seither
R
,
Loretan
C
,
Driver
K
,
Mellerson
JL
,
Knighton
CL
,
Black
CL
.
Vaccination coverage with selected vaccines and exemption rates among children in kindergarten - United States, 2018–19 school year
.
MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep
.
2019
;
68
:
905
912
54
Wang
Y
,
Hartman
M
,
Aziz
NAA
,
Arora
S
,
Shi
L
,
Tunison
E
.
A systematic review of the use of LENA technology
.
Am Ann Deaf
.
2017
;
162
(
3
):
295
311
55
Peterson
JW
,
Huffman
LC
,
Bruce
J
,
Prata
N
,
Harley
KG
,
Chamberlain
LJ
.
A clinic-based school readiness coaching intervention for low-income Latino children: an intervention study
.
Clin Pediatr (Phila)
.
2020
;
59
(
14
):
1240
1251
56
Peterson
JW
,
Almanzar
N
,
Chamberlain
LJ
, et al
.
School readiness coaching in the pediatric clinic: Latinx parent perspectives
.
Acad Pediatr
.
2021
;
21
(
5
):
802
808
57
Bruce
JS
,
De La Cruz
MM
,
Moreno
G
,
Chamberlain
LJ
.
Lunch at the library: examination of a community-based approach to addressing summer food insecurity
.
Public Health Nutr
.
2017
;
20
(
9
):
1640
1649
58
Silverstein
M
,
Mack
C
,
Reavis
N
,
Koepsell
TD
,
Gross
GS
,
Grossman
DC
.
Effect of a clinic-based referral system to head start: a randomized controlled trial
.
JAMA
.
2004
;
292
(
8
):
968
971
59
Help Me Grow
.
The HMG system model
.
60
Peterson
JW
,
Loeb
S
,
Chamberlain
LJ
.
The Intersection of health and education to address school readiness of all children
.
Pediatrics
.
2018
;
142
(
5
):
e20181126
61
National Science Foundation
.
Convergence research at NSF
.
Available at: https://www.nsf.gov/od/oia/convergence/index.jsp. Accessed August 13, 2021

Competing Interests

POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.