Little attention has been given to the study of early childhood factors that protect against the development of obesity and severe obesity. We investigated whether exposure to familial psychosocial assets and risks in infancy (1–15 months) and early childhood (24–54 months) and child behavioral regulation in early childhood predict longitudinal change in BMI (2 to 15 years).
Participants included 1077 predominantly non-Hispanic, White, English-speaking mother-child dyads from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development dataset. Cumulative familial asset and risk indices were created using measures (eg, maternal parenting sensitivity, poverty) from 2 developmental periods (1–15 months, 24–54 months). A child behavioral regulation index was created on the basis of behavioral tasks and parent reports. Previously published BMI trajectories (nonoverweight [40th percentile], nonoverweight [70th percentile], overweight/obese, severely obese) were used as the outcome.
All indices predicted membership in the overweight/obese trajectory; however, when entered into the same model, only familial assets continued to reduce the odds of membership in this trajectory. Familial assets and child behavioral regulation independently reduced the odds of membership in the severely obese trajectory. Furthermore, child behavioral regulation and familial assets buffered the negative effects of familial risk on BMI trajectory membership.
Early exposure to familial assets and child behavioral regulation may have long-term protective effects on weight gain over early exposure to some familial risk factors (eg, poverty); thus, these indices may help foster obesity resilience.
One in 3 youth are overweight or obese, and 5% to 9% are severely obese. Few studies have identified protective familial and child assets against the development of obesity and severe obesity in childhood, a sensitive period of human development.
Early exposure to familial psychosocial assets and child behavioral regulation independently reduced children’s risk for obesity and severe obesity and buffered the negative effects of familial risk on BMI trajectory membership.
One in 3 youth had overweight or obesity in 2016; 5% to 9% of youth had severe obesity.1 Pediatric obesity is defined as having a BMI (measured in kilograms per meters squared) ≥95th percentile; severe obesity is defined as having a BMI ≥120% of the 95th percentile.1,2 Children’s weight outcomes are shaped by multiple levels of influence, including family characteristics (eg, parenting styles) and child behaviors (eg, behavioral regulation), interacting at multiples stages of child development.3–6 To date, researchers have primarily focused on identifying risk factors for childhood obesity, such as exposure to adversity (eg, household poverty) early in life.7–9 Strength-based approaches to identifying factors that promote obesity resilience (eg, the process of maintaining a healthy weight despite being exposed to circumstances that contribute to the development of obesity10 ) have been underused. Evidence suggests that familial and child psychosocial and behavioral assets may contribute to obesity resilience. Responsive parenting practices (characterized by warmth and responsiveness to distress) have been shown to reduce obesity risk in childhood11–13 and to buffer the negative effects of early adversity on developmental outcomes.14 Children who score higher in self-regulatory capacity show lower intakes of energy-dense foods15 and reduced risk for weight gain.16,17
The current study used a life course approach, an intellectual framework that illustrates how individual behaviors, timing of exposure, and sensitive periods, and social context facilitate or hinder developmental outcomes.18,19 With this approach, we investigated whether exposure to family assets during 2 sensitive periods of human development,20 infancy (1–15 months) and early childhood (24–54 months), and child behavioral assets in early childhood promote resilience against the development of obesity and severe obesity from childhood to adolescence among a large US sample of predominantly non-Hispanic, White, English-speaking mother-child dyads. We hypothesized that assets would (1) inversely predict membership in obese and severely obese BMI trajectories and (2) buffer (ie, moderate) the negative effects of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership.
Methods
Participants
Data were drawn from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD SECCYD)21,22,23,24 ; study details have been previously published.25 A total of 1364 families with full-term, healthy newborns were recruited from hospitals at 10 data collection sites in 1991. Families were excluded if (1) mothers were <18 years of age, (2) newborns had obvious disabilities at birth or stayed in the hospital >7 days after birth, (3) mothers did not sufficiently converse in English, or (4) families anticipated moving from the catchment area within 3 years. Home and laboratory visits were completed at 1, 6, 15, 24, 36, and 54 months and 7, 9, 11, 12, 13, 14, and 15 years of age. Attrition across the course of the study resulted in a sample of 1009 participants at 15 years. Only children having BMI trajectories (described below) were retained in the current study (n = 1077; a loss of 287 participants). As described previously26 and for purposes of modeling normative growth patterns, the 287 participants were excluded for the following reasons: gestational age <37 or >42 weeks (n = 119), <2 BMI data points between 2 and 15 years (n = 165), or implausible BMI data (n = 3). Included youth did not differ from those excluded (n = 287) in race (White versus child of color; P = .142). Included youth were more likely to be girls (P = .052), reside in 2-parent households (P = .007), have higher income-to-needs ratios (P = .010), and have mothers with ≥16 years of education (P = .0001).
Measures
We used 4 BMI trajectory groups previously identified in the NICHD SECCYD sample, using latent growth curve modeling as our categorical outcome variable.26 For brevity, children’s weight and standing height were measured in duplicate and used to calculate age- and sex-specific BMI indices.27 Overweight, according to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention,27 is defined as BMI ≥85th percentile, class I obesity as BMI ≥95th percentile, class II obesity (severe obesity) as BMI ≥120% of 95th percentile, and class III obesity (severe obesity) as BMI ≥140% of 95th percentile. Each BMI trajectory group was defined on the basis of children’s growth from 2 to 15 years. The largest BMI trajectory, referred to as nonoverweight (40th percentile) (n = 468 of 1077; 43.4%), had an average BMI percentile of 40.3 between 2 and 15 years. The second trajectory, nonoverweight (70th percentile) (n = 383; 35.6%), had an average BMI percentile of 72.8 between 2 and 15 years. All youth in these 2 nonoverweight groups had BMI percentiles <85th at each time point. The third trajectory, overweight/obese (n = 157; 14.6%), had average BMI percentiles exceeding the cutoff for overweight between 5 and 15 years, and the cutoff for obesity class I between 11 and 15 years. The final group, severely obese (n = 69, 6.4%), had average BMI percentiles exceeding the cutoff for obesity class II (severe obesity) at all time points between 5 and 15 years, indicating a persistent prevalence of severe obesity.
Table 1 provides the measures, time of assessment, and scoring for each cumulative index of familial assets and risks and child behavioral regulation; the Supplemental Information provides additional details on scoring and psychometrics. For example, the familial assets index (1–15 months) consisted of 3 protective factors (eg, maternal sensitivity) and was created in 3 steps. In step 1, maternal sensitivity scores at 6 and 15 months were averaged for each participant to create a mean score for infancy. In step 2, this mean score was dichotomized; scores in the highest quartile (ie, >75th percentile) were coded as 1; lower scores were coded as 0. Because this measure does not have preexisting cutoffs for high scores, we used quartile splits to identify the highest scores, based on the sample distribution. After these 2 steps were repeated for the remaining 2 familial assets (ie, maternal education, home enrichment), in the third and final step, all 3 dichotomized familial assets were summed to create a cumulative index for infancy (range, 0–3). The same 3-step process was repeated for the familial asset index (24–54 months), familial risk indices (range, 0–6), and child behavioral regulation index (range, 0–3).
Measures by Domain . | Developmental Period, Months . | Instrument/Description . | Exposure Coded as 1 . | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Infancy . | Preschool Years . | |||||||
1 . | 6 . | 15 . | 24 . | 36 . | 54 . | |||
Familial assets | ||||||||
Mom sensitivity | X | X | X | X | X | Assessed using 15 min of semistructured mother-child free play videotaped during a laboratory visit20,21 | Scores >75th percentilea | |
H.O.M.E. Inventory | X | X | X | X | H.O.M.E. Inventory,28 a measure capturing the quality and extent of stimulation available to a child in the home environment | Scores >75th percentilea | ||
Maternal education, ≥16 y | X | Mother reported | ≥16 y | |||||
Child behavioral regulation | ||||||||
Inhibitory control | X | Children’s Behavior Questionnaire29 | Scores >75th percentilea | |||||
Self-control toy task | X | Video-recorded self-control task measured children’s ability to delay or inhibit play with an appealing, forbidden toy. Each child was left alone with the target toy for 150 s, and latency to first active engagement was coded; interrater reliability was 0.92. | Waited ≥75 s before touching the forbidden toy | |||||
Delay of gratification task | X | Video-recorded assessment16 of children’s ability to control their impulses and wait for a larger reward. In this task, children had the opportunity to choose between an immediate, small food reward (eg, mini chocolate bar) and a larger, but less immediate reward (eg, large chocolate bar) that they would have to wait 7 min to receive. | Waited for the larger reward | |||||
Familial risks | ||||||||
Poverty | X | X | X | X | X | Mother-reported household demographics, including income, and household size and structure | INR ≤1 | |
Single-parent household | X | X | X | X | X | X | Mother-reported household demographics, including income, and household size and structure | Single-parent household = 1 |
Maternal depression symptomology | X | X | X | X | X | X | Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression30 | Scores ≥16 |
Maternal life events | X | X | X | X | Developed by NICHD SECCYD investigators; sum of exposure to 7 stressful life events (eg, maternal job loss and household moves) in past 3 mo31 | Scores >75th percentilea | ||
Maternal general health status | X | X | X | X | Developed by NICHD SECCYD investigators; mothers asked, “How would you describe your health in the last 3 mo, compared with other women your age?” (1 = poor, 2 = fair, 3 = good, and 4 = excellent) | Scores >75th percentilea | ||
Parenting stress | X | X | X | X | X | Parent Role Quality Scale32 ; mother report of the concerning/rewarding parts of their experience as a parent | Scores >75th percentilea |
Measures by Domain . | Developmental Period, Months . | Instrument/Description . | Exposure Coded as 1 . | |||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Infancy . | Preschool Years . | |||||||
1 . | 6 . | 15 . | 24 . | 36 . | 54 . | |||
Familial assets | ||||||||
Mom sensitivity | X | X | X | X | X | Assessed using 15 min of semistructured mother-child free play videotaped during a laboratory visit20,21 | Scores >75th percentilea | |
H.O.M.E. Inventory | X | X | X | X | H.O.M.E. Inventory,28 a measure capturing the quality and extent of stimulation available to a child in the home environment | Scores >75th percentilea | ||
Maternal education, ≥16 y | X | Mother reported | ≥16 y | |||||
Child behavioral regulation | ||||||||
Inhibitory control | X | Children’s Behavior Questionnaire29 | Scores >75th percentilea | |||||
Self-control toy task | X | Video-recorded self-control task measured children’s ability to delay or inhibit play with an appealing, forbidden toy. Each child was left alone with the target toy for 150 s, and latency to first active engagement was coded; interrater reliability was 0.92. | Waited ≥75 s before touching the forbidden toy | |||||
Delay of gratification task | X | Video-recorded assessment16 of children’s ability to control their impulses and wait for a larger reward. In this task, children had the opportunity to choose between an immediate, small food reward (eg, mini chocolate bar) and a larger, but less immediate reward (eg, large chocolate bar) that they would have to wait 7 min to receive. | Waited for the larger reward | |||||
Familial risks | ||||||||
Poverty | X | X | X | X | X | Mother-reported household demographics, including income, and household size and structure | INR ≤1 | |
Single-parent household | X | X | X | X | X | X | Mother-reported household demographics, including income, and household size and structure | Single-parent household = 1 |
Maternal depression symptomology | X | X | X | X | X | X | Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression30 | Scores ≥16 |
Maternal life events | X | X | X | X | Developed by NICHD SECCYD investigators; sum of exposure to 7 stressful life events (eg, maternal job loss and household moves) in past 3 mo31 | Scores >75th percentilea | ||
Maternal general health status | X | X | X | X | Developed by NICHD SECCYD investigators; mothers asked, “How would you describe your health in the last 3 mo, compared with other women your age?” (1 = poor, 2 = fair, 3 = good, and 4 = excellent) | Scores >75th percentilea | ||
Parenting stress | X | X | X | X | X | Parent Role Quality Scale32 ; mother report of the concerning/rewarding parts of their experience as a parent | Scores >75th percentilea |
H.O.M.E., Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment; INR, income-to-needs ratio.
Scores >75th percentile, based on the distribution for the current sample, were coded as 1; scores <75th percentile were coded as 0.
At the 1-month home visit, mothers reported household and child demographics (eg, child race/ethnicity). Youth were 77.5% non-Hispanic White; 11.9% were non-Hispanic Black; 5.9% were Hispanic; 3.3% were other; and <3% were American Indian, Asian, or Pacific Islander. Child race/ethnicity was coded as 1 for child of color (22.5%) and 0 for non-Hispanic White (77.5%), given the small proportions of participants of color. Tanner staging criteria33 were assessed at age 11 years by nurse practitioners to account for the obesity-puberty confounds.34 Boys’ pubertal status was calculated by averaging pubic hair and genital Tanner stages; girls’ pubertal status was calculated by averaging breast and pubic hair Tanner stages. Although puberty data were available at the 9-, 10-, 11-, and 12-year assessments, ages 10 and 11 had the highest level of variability (41.2 and 39.8, respectively); age 11 data were selected because of fewer missing data points (27% vs 33.2%).
Statistical Analysis
Guided by best practices,35 25 multiple imputations were performed by using SAS version 9.4 statistical software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC). All study variables, sociodemographic data, BMI data, and BMI trajectory groups were included (before subscale creation) to inform the imputation. Only 4.9% of data points were missing across the entire sample for the variables of interest. Supplemental Table 12 provides the imputed and nonimputed descriptive statistics, and percent missing for all study variables. The highest level of missing data was observed for pubertal status at 11 years (27.9%); thus, we included pubertal status at 9, 10, 11, and 12 years to inform the imputation. Parameter estimates were combined across imputations using PROC MI ANALYZE. Pearson intercorrelations were run between familial and child cumulative indices. First, bivariate multinomial logistic regression models were run to test for the independent effects of each predictor (ie, familial risk [1–15 months, 24–54 months], familial assets [1–15 months, 24–54 months] child behavioral regulation [36–54 months]) on BMI trajectory membership. Second, multivariate multinomial logistic regression models were run to test whether the indices made unique contributions to predicting BMI trajectory membership as follows: model 1, both familial risk indices; model 2, both familial asset indices; model 3, familial risk and asset indices (1–15 months); and model 4, familial risk and asset indices (24–54 months) and child behavioral regulation. Third, in our final step, we investigated whether familial assets and child behavioral regulation moderated the effects of familial risk on child BMI trajectory membership (models 5–8). Given the small size of the heavier BMI trajectories/groups, moderators showing at least marginal significance (P < .10) were explored. To interpret interaction terms, the sample was divided into groups using median splits on the moderator (eg, low versus high behavioral regulation); multinomial models were run again with this grouping variable as a dichotomous predictor in place of the original continuous variable. All models were run with covariates (eg, child race [1 = child of color, 0 = White], sex [1 = female, 0 = male], and pubertal status). Because the familial risk and assets indices included household sociodemographic measures (eg, poverty status), these measures were not used as covariates.
Results
Sample descriptives are presented in Table 2. As shown in Table 3, familial risks and assets tracked well over time. Familial assets and child behavioral regulation were correlated in a positive direction, and both were inversely associated with familial risk.
. | BMI Trajectory . | Total Sample . | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
. | Nonoverweight(40th Percentile) . | Nonoverweight(70th Percentile) . | Overweight/Obese . | Severely Obese . | |
No. (%) | 468 (43.4) | 383 (35.6) | 157 (14.5) | 69 (6.4) | 1077 (100.0) |
Sociodemographic characteristics at birth | |||||
Child sex, % female | 51.9 | 47.5 | 50.3 | 46.3 | 49.7 |
Child race, % | |||||
White (non-Hispanic) | 78.8 | 78.3 | 74.5 | 71.0 | 77.5 |
Black (non-Hispanic) | 9.8 | 11.5 | 14.6 | 21.7 | 11.9 |
Othera | 11.4 | 10.2 | 10.9 | 7.3 | 10.6 |
Household INR (1 mo), mean % (95% CI) | 3.0 (2.7–3.3) | 2.9 (2.6–3.2) | 2.5 (2.1–2.9) | 2.0 (1.4–2.6) | 2.9 (2.7–3.0) |
Maternal education, y | 14.6 | 14.4 | 13.8 | 13.3 | 14.4 |
Single-parent household (1 mo), mean % (95% CI) | 10.5 (7.7–13.3) | 13.5 (11.1–16.9) | 15.3 (9.6–20.8) | 23.1 (12.1–33.1) | 13.1 (11.1–15.1) |
Puberty status, mean % (95% CI) | |||||
Pubertal status (11 y) | 2.1 (2.0–2.2) | 2.3 (2.2–2.4) | 2.4 (2.2–2.5) | 2.5 (2.2–2.7) | 2.2 (2.2-2.3) |
Predictors, mean % (95% CI) | |||||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | 1.6 (1.5–1.7) | 1.5 (1.4–1.6) | 1.3 (1.1–1.5) | 0.9 (0.6–1.1) | 1.5 (1.4–1.5) |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | 1.4 (1.2–1.5) | 1.2 (1.1–1.4) | 0.9 (0.7–1.1) | 0.5 (0.3–0.8) | 1.2 (1.1–1.3) |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | 1.5 (1.4–1.6) | 1.4 (1.3–1.5) | 1.3 (1.1–1.4) | 0.9 (0.7–1.2) | 1.4 (1.3–1.4) |
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | 1.7 (1.6–1.8) | 1.8 (1.7–1.9) | 2.0 (1.8–2.2) | 2.0 (1.7–2.4) | 1.8 (1.7–1.9) |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | 1.7 (1.6–1.8) | 1.9 (1.7–2.0) | 2.1 (1.8–2.3) | 1.9 (1.6–2.3) | 1.8 (1.7–1.9) |
. | BMI Trajectory . | Total Sample . | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
. | Nonoverweight(40th Percentile) . | Nonoverweight(70th Percentile) . | Overweight/Obese . | Severely Obese . | |
No. (%) | 468 (43.4) | 383 (35.6) | 157 (14.5) | 69 (6.4) | 1077 (100.0) |
Sociodemographic characteristics at birth | |||||
Child sex, % female | 51.9 | 47.5 | 50.3 | 46.3 | 49.7 |
Child race, % | |||||
White (non-Hispanic) | 78.8 | 78.3 | 74.5 | 71.0 | 77.5 |
Black (non-Hispanic) | 9.8 | 11.5 | 14.6 | 21.7 | 11.9 |
Othera | 11.4 | 10.2 | 10.9 | 7.3 | 10.6 |
Household INR (1 mo), mean % (95% CI) | 3.0 (2.7–3.3) | 2.9 (2.6–3.2) | 2.5 (2.1–2.9) | 2.0 (1.4–2.6) | 2.9 (2.7–3.0) |
Maternal education, y | 14.6 | 14.4 | 13.8 | 13.3 | 14.4 |
Single-parent household (1 mo), mean % (95% CI) | 10.5 (7.7–13.3) | 13.5 (11.1–16.9) | 15.3 (9.6–20.8) | 23.1 (12.1–33.1) | 13.1 (11.1–15.1) |
Puberty status, mean % (95% CI) | |||||
Pubertal status (11 y) | 2.1 (2.0–2.2) | 2.3 (2.2–2.4) | 2.4 (2.2–2.5) | 2.5 (2.2–2.7) | 2.2 (2.2-2.3) |
Predictors, mean % (95% CI) | |||||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | 1.6 (1.5–1.7) | 1.5 (1.4–1.6) | 1.3 (1.1–1.5) | 0.9 (0.6–1.1) | 1.5 (1.4–1.5) |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | 1.4 (1.2–1.5) | 1.2 (1.1–1.4) | 0.9 (0.7–1.1) | 0.5 (0.3–0.8) | 1.2 (1.1–1.3) |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | 1.5 (1.4–1.6) | 1.4 (1.3–1.5) | 1.3 (1.1–1.4) | 0.9 (0.7–1.2) | 1.4 (1.3–1.4) |
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | 1.7 (1.6–1.8) | 1.8 (1.7–1.9) | 2.0 (1.8–2.2) | 2.0 (1.7–2.4) | 1.8 (1.7–1.9) |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | 1.7 (1.6–1.8) | 1.9 (1.7–2.0) | 2.1 (1.8–2.3) | 1.9 (1.6–2.3) | 1.8 (1.7–1.9) |
CI, confidence interval; INR, income-to-needs ratio.
Mothers reported on their child’s race and ethnicity using the following categories: (1) American Indian, Eskimo, Aleut; (2) Asian or Pacific Islander; (3) Black or African American; (4) White; and (5) other; they were also asked to indicate whether their child was Hispanic. These data were used to classify children into the following race/ethnic categories: non-Hispanic White, non-Hispanic Black, and other.
. | 1 . | 2 . | 3 . | 4 . | 5 . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Familial assets (1–15 mo) | 1.00 | ||||
2. Familial assets (24–54 mo) | 0.68*** | 1.00 | |||
3. Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | 0.29*** | 0.36*** | 1.00 | ||
4. Familial risks (1–15 mo) | −0.35*** | −0.33*** | −0.24*** | 1.00 | |
5. Familial risks (24–54 mo) | −0.30*** | −0.33*** | −0.23*** | 0.50*** | 1.00 |
. | 1 . | 2 . | 3 . | 4 . | 5 . |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Familial assets (1–15 mo) | 1.00 | ||||
2. Familial assets (24–54 mo) | 0.68*** | 1.00 | |||
3. Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | 0.29*** | 0.36*** | 1.00 | ||
4. Familial risks (1–15 mo) | −0.35*** | −0.33*** | −0.24*** | 1.00 | |
5. Familial risks (24–54 mo) | −0.30*** | −0.33*** | −0.23*** | 0.50*** | 1.00 |
P < .001.
Predicting BMI Trajectories
Adjusted odds ratios (AORs) for all 5 cumulative indices predicting BMI trajectory membership are listed in Table 4, with nonoverweight (40th percentile) as the reference group. Results were similar when nonoverweight (70th percentile) served as the reference group and, thus, are not reported. Because we were interested in identifying factors that distinguish children who become severely obese from those who do not, we also present AORs with the overweight/obese trajectory as the reference group in Table 5. The results in Tables 4 and 5 are presented as bivariate models (evaluating independent effects of each predictor), multivariate models (presenting unique effects of each predictor in the presence of other predictors), and moderation models (presenting moderating effects of familial and child assets on the link between familial risks and BMI trajectories).
. | BMI Trajectory, AOR (95% CI) . | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nonoverweight (40th Percentile) . | Nonoverweight (70th Percentile) . | Overweight/Obese . | Severely Obese . | ||
Bivariate models | |||||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.99 (0.846–1.14) | 0.81 (0.67–0.99)* | 0.50 (0.35–0.71)*** | |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.93 (0.80–1.07) | 0.69 (0.56–0.85)*** | 0.47 (0.33–0.67)*** | |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.89 (0.76–1.04) | 0.79 (0.64–0.98)* | 0.57 (0.42–0.77)*** | |
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.04 (0.92–1.16) | 1.16 (1.01–1.34)* | 1.16 (0.94–1.42) | |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 1.12 (1.01–1.25)* | 1.24 (1.07–1.43)*** | 1.13 (0.92–1.39) | |
Multivariate models: both developmental periods | |||||
Model 1 | |||||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.09 (0.89–1.33) | 1.11 (0.84–1.47) | 0.74 (0.46–1.19) | |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.88 (0.72–1.07) | 0.64 (0.48–0.86)** | 0.58 (0.36–0.93)* | |
Model 2 | |||||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.98 (0.86–1.10) | 1.00 (0.90–1.26) | 1.12 (0.89–1.41) | |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 1.13 (1.01–1.29)* | 1.20 (1.02–1.42)* | 1.07 (0.85–1.35) | |
Multivariate models: infancy/toddlerhood | |||||
Model 3 | |||||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.04 (0.92–1.16) | 1.12 (0.96–1.30) | 0.99 (0.80–1.22) | |
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.00 (0.86–1.16) | 0.85 (0.69–1.04) | 0.50 (0.35–0.73)*** | |
Multivariate models: preschool years | |||||
Model 4 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 1.11 (0.98–1.24) | 1.15 (0.99–1.34) | 0.97 (0.79–1.20) | |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.98 (0.84–1.14) | 0.75 (0.60–0.93)** | 0.52 (0.36–0.76)*** | |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.92 (0.78–1.10) | 0.90 (0.72–1.14) | 0.66 (0.47–0.91)** | |
Interaction model: infancy/toddlerhood | |||||
Does exposure to familial assets in infancy/toddlerhood moderate the effects of concurrent familial risk on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?a | |||||
Model 5 | |||||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) × familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.05 (0.92–1.19) | 1.03 (0.86–1.24) | 0.67 (0.46–0.98)* | |
Simple effects of familial risk (1–15 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (1–15 mo) | |||||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.05 (0.90–1.22) | 1.11 (0.93–1.34) | 1.14 (0.91–1.43) | |
High familial assets | Ref | 1.07 (0.89–1.29) | 1.10 (0.83–1.44) | 0.59 (0.34–1.04)† | |
Interaction models: Preschool years | |||||
Does exposure to familial assets in early childhood moderate the effects of concurrent familial risk on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?b | |||||
Model 6 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.96 (0.84–1.09) | 1.05 (0.87–1.28) | 0.65 (0.45–0.95)* | |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (24–54 mo) | |||||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.17 (1.00–1.36) | 1.13 (0.94–1.37) | 1.17 (0.92–1.48) | |
High familial assets | Ref | 1.02 (0.85–1.24) | 1.24 (0.93–1.66) | 0.47 (0.27–0.84)** | |
Does child behavioral regulation moderate the effect of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?c | |||||
Model 7 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.94 (0.83–1.06) | 0.87 (0.74–1.01)† | 0.80 (0.63–1.01)† | |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | |||||
Low child behavioral reg. | Ref | 1.18 (1.01–1.24)* | 1.39 (1.15–1.69)*** | 1.24 (0.97–1.48)† | |
High child behavioral reg. | Ref | 1.04 (0.87–1.24) | 0.99 (0.77–1.27) | 0.73 (0.44–1.19) | |
Does child behavioral regulation continue to moderate the effect of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age), after adjusting for familial assets?d | |||||
Model 8 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.94 (0.83–1.06) | 0.86 (0.74–1.01)† | 0.79 (0.63–1.01)† | |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, moderated by levels of child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | |||||
Low child behavioral regulation | Ref | 1.17 (1.01–1.37)* | 1.39 (1.15–1.69)* | 1.24(0.97–1.48)† | |
High child behavioral regulation | Ref | 1.03 (0.86–1.24) | 0.94 (0.73–1.21) | 0.67 (0.42–1.08) |
. | BMI Trajectory, AOR (95% CI) . | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Nonoverweight (40th Percentile) . | Nonoverweight (70th Percentile) . | Overweight/Obese . | Severely Obese . | ||
Bivariate models | |||||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.99 (0.846–1.14) | 0.81 (0.67–0.99)* | 0.50 (0.35–0.71)*** | |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.93 (0.80–1.07) | 0.69 (0.56–0.85)*** | 0.47 (0.33–0.67)*** | |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.89 (0.76–1.04) | 0.79 (0.64–0.98)* | 0.57 (0.42–0.77)*** | |
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.04 (0.92–1.16) | 1.16 (1.01–1.34)* | 1.16 (0.94–1.42) | |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 1.12 (1.01–1.25)* | 1.24 (1.07–1.43)*** | 1.13 (0.92–1.39) | |
Multivariate models: both developmental periods | |||||
Model 1 | |||||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.09 (0.89–1.33) | 1.11 (0.84–1.47) | 0.74 (0.46–1.19) | |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.88 (0.72–1.07) | 0.64 (0.48–0.86)** | 0.58 (0.36–0.93)* | |
Model 2 | |||||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.98 (0.86–1.10) | 1.00 (0.90–1.26) | 1.12 (0.89–1.41) | |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 1.13 (1.01–1.29)* | 1.20 (1.02–1.42)* | 1.07 (0.85–1.35) | |
Multivariate models: infancy/toddlerhood | |||||
Model 3 | |||||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.04 (0.92–1.16) | 1.12 (0.96–1.30) | 0.99 (0.80–1.22) | |
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.00 (0.86–1.16) | 0.85 (0.69–1.04) | 0.50 (0.35–0.73)*** | |
Multivariate models: preschool years | |||||
Model 4 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 1.11 (0.98–1.24) | 1.15 (0.99–1.34) | 0.97 (0.79–1.20) | |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.98 (0.84–1.14) | 0.75 (0.60–0.93)** | 0.52 (0.36–0.76)*** | |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.92 (0.78–1.10) | 0.90 (0.72–1.14) | 0.66 (0.47–0.91)** | |
Interaction model: infancy/toddlerhood | |||||
Does exposure to familial assets in infancy/toddlerhood moderate the effects of concurrent familial risk on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?a | |||||
Model 5 | |||||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) × familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.05 (0.92–1.19) | 1.03 (0.86–1.24) | 0.67 (0.46–0.98)* | |
Simple effects of familial risk (1–15 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (1–15 mo) | |||||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.05 (0.90–1.22) | 1.11 (0.93–1.34) | 1.14 (0.91–1.43) | |
High familial assets | Ref | 1.07 (0.89–1.29) | 1.10 (0.83–1.44) | 0.59 (0.34–1.04)† | |
Interaction models: Preschool years | |||||
Does exposure to familial assets in early childhood moderate the effects of concurrent familial risk on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?b | |||||
Model 6 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.96 (0.84–1.09) | 1.05 (0.87–1.28) | 0.65 (0.45–0.95)* | |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (24–54 mo) | |||||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.17 (1.00–1.36) | 1.13 (0.94–1.37) | 1.17 (0.92–1.48) | |
High familial assets | Ref | 1.02 (0.85–1.24) | 1.24 (0.93–1.66) | 0.47 (0.27–0.84)** | |
Does child behavioral regulation moderate the effect of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?c | |||||
Model 7 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.94 (0.83–1.06) | 0.87 (0.74–1.01)† | 0.80 (0.63–1.01)† | |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | |||||
Low child behavioral reg. | Ref | 1.18 (1.01–1.24)* | 1.39 (1.15–1.69)*** | 1.24 (0.97–1.48)† | |
High child behavioral reg. | Ref | 1.04 (0.87–1.24) | 0.99 (0.77–1.27) | 0.73 (0.44–1.19) | |
Does child behavioral regulation continue to moderate the effect of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age), after adjusting for familial assets?d | |||||
Model 8 | |||||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.94 (0.83–1.06) | 0.86 (0.74–1.01)† | 0.79 (0.63–1.01)† | |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, moderated by levels of child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | |||||
Low child behavioral regulation | Ref | 1.17 (1.01–1.37)* | 1.39 (1.15–1.69)* | 1.24(0.97–1.48)† | |
High child behavioral regulation | Ref | 1.03 (0.86–1.24) | 0.94 (0.73–1.21) | 0.67 (0.42–1.08) |
AORs adjusted for child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status. CI, confidence interval; Ref, reference group.
Model 5 included the main effects for familial risk (1–15 mo) and familial assets (1–15 mo.); the interaction term for familial risk (1–15 mo) × familial assets (1–15 mo); and the following study covariates: child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
Model 6 included the main effects for familial risk (24–54 mo) and familial assets (24–54 mo); the interaction term for familial risk (24–54 mo) × familial assets (24–54 mo); and the following study covariates: child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
Model 7 included the main effects for familial risk (24–54 mo) and child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo.); the interaction term for familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo.); and the following study covariates: child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
Model 8 included the main effects for familial assets (24–54 mo), familial risk (24–54 mo), and child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo); the interaction term for familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo); and the following study covariates: child race (1 = children of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
P < .05.
P < .01.
P < .001.
P < .10.
. | BMI Trajectory, AOR (95% CI) . | |
---|---|---|
Overweight/Obese . | Severely Obese . | |
Bivariate models | ||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.61 (0.42–0.90)** |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.68 (0.46–0.99)* |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.70 (0.49–0.99)* |
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.00 (0.80–1.25) |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.92 (0.73–1.15) |
Multivariate models: both developmental periods | ||
Model 1 | ||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.67 (0.40–1.11) |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.90 (0.53–1.50) |
Model 2 | ||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.05 (0.82–1.36) |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.89 (0.69–1.15) |
Multivariate models: infancy/toddlerhood | ||
Model 3 | ||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.92 (0.74–1.16) |
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.59 (0.40–0.88)** |
Multivariate models: preschool years | ||
Model 4 | ||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.84 (0.67–1.05) |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.70 (0.47–1.05) |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.73 (0.50–1.05) |
Interaction model: infancy/toddlerhood | ||
Does exposure to familial assets in infancy/toddlerhood moderate the effects of familial risk (also during infancy) on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?a | ||
Model 5 | ||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) × familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.69 (0.47–1.03)† |
Simple effects of familial risk (1–15 mo.) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (1–15 mo) | ||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.02 (0.80–1.32) |
High familial assets | Ref | 0.54 (0.30–0.98)* |
Interaction models: preschool years | ||
Does exposure to familial assets in early childhood moderate the effects of concurrent familial risk on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?b | ||
Model 6 | ||
Familial risk (24–54mo) × familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.62 (0.41–0.92)* |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (24–54 mo) | ||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.03 (0.80–1.32) |
High familial assets | Ref | 0.38 (0.21–0.70)*** |
Does child behavioral regulation moderate the effect of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?c | ||
Model 7 | ||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.94 (0.74–1.20) |
. | BMI Trajectory, AOR (95% CI) . | |
---|---|---|
Overweight/Obese . | Severely Obese . | |
Bivariate models | ||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.61 (0.42–0.90)** |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.68 (0.46–0.99)* |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.70 (0.49–0.99)* |
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.00 (0.80–1.25) |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.92 (0.73–1.15) |
Multivariate models: both developmental periods | ||
Model 1 | ||
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.67 (0.40–1.11) |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.90 (0.53–1.50) |
Model 2 | ||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 1.05 (0.82–1.36) |
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.89 (0.69–1.15) |
Multivariate models: infancy/toddlerhood | ||
Model 3 | ||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.92 (0.74–1.16) |
Familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.59 (0.40–0.88)** |
Multivariate models: preschool years | ||
Model 4 | ||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.84 (0.67–1.05) |
Familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.70 (0.47–1.05) |
Child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.73 (0.50–1.05) |
Interaction model: infancy/toddlerhood | ||
Does exposure to familial assets in infancy/toddlerhood moderate the effects of familial risk (also during infancy) on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?a | ||
Model 5 | ||
Familial risk (1–15 mo) × familial assets (1–15 mo) | Ref | 0.69 (0.47–1.03)† |
Simple effects of familial risk (1–15 mo.) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (1–15 mo) | ||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.02 (0.80–1.32) |
High familial assets | Ref | 0.54 (0.30–0.98)* |
Interaction models: preschool years | ||
Does exposure to familial assets in early childhood moderate the effects of concurrent familial risk on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?b | ||
Model 6 | ||
Familial risk (24–54mo) × familial assets (24–54 mo) | Ref | 0.62 (0.41–0.92)* |
Simple effects of familial risk (24–54 mo) → BMI trajectory, by levels of familial assets (24–54 mo) | ||
Low familial assets | Ref | 1.03 (0.80–1.32) |
High familial assets | Ref | 0.38 (0.21–0.70)*** |
Does child behavioral regulation moderate the effect of familial risks on BMI trajectory membership (2 to 15 years of age)?c | ||
Model 7 | ||
Familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo) | Ref | 0.94 (0.74–1.20) |
AORs adjusted for child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status. CI, confidence interval; Ref, reference group.
Model 5 included the main effects for familial risk (1–15 mo) and familial assets (1–15 mo); the interaction term for familial risk (1–15 mo) × familial assets (1–15 mo), and the following study covariates: child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
Model 6 included the main effects for familial risk (24–54 mo) and familial assets (24–54 mo); the interaction term for familial risk (24–54 mo) × familial assets (24–54 mo); and the following study covariates: child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
Model 7 included the main effects for familial risk (24–54 mo) and child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo); the interaction term for familial risk (24–54 mo) × child behavioral regulation (36–54 mo); and the following study covariates: child race (1 = child of color, 0 = White), child sex (1 = female, 0 = male), and child puberty status.
P < .05.
P < .01.
P < .001.
P < .10.
Nonoverweight (70th Percentile) BMI Trajectory
As shown in Table 4, children exposed to greater familial risk during 24 to 54 months had an increased odds of being in the nonoverweight (70th percentile) trajectory compared with the nonoverweight (40th percentile) trajectory. This effect became nonsignificant after familial assets and child behavioral regulation were added in subsequent models.
Overweight/Obese BMI Trajectory
As shown in Table 4 (bivariate models), greater exposure to familial assets during both developmental periods and higher child behavioral regulation decreased the odds of being in the overweight/obese trajectory, whereas familial risk increased the odds relative to the nonoverweight (40th percentile) trajectory. Only familial assets (24–54 months) continued to remain statistically significant after both familial asset indices were entered into the same model (model 1) and after child behavioral regulation and familial risk (24–54 months) were added in model 4.
Among the moderation models in Table 4, the interaction term for familial risk by child behavioral regulation (24–54 months; model 7) predicted membership in the overweight/obese trajectory. As shown, among children with low behavioral regulation, greater exposure to familial risks (24–54 months) predicted membership in the overweight/obese group, whereas no relation was observed among children with high behavioral regulation. This result remained unchanged after adjusting for familial risk (model 8). It is noteworthy that a similar moderation effect was observed for the nonoverweight (70th percentile) BMI trajectory, despite the interaction term not reaching statistical significance for this group (simple effects are shown in Table 4).
Severely Obese BMI Trajectory
As shown in Tables 4 and 5 (bivariate models), greater exposure to familial assets during both developmental periods and higher child behavioral regulation decreased the odds of being in the severely obese trajectory compared with the nonoverweight (40th percentile) and overweight/obese trajectories. No effects were observed for familial risk indices. Both familial assets (24–54 months) and child behavioral regulation continued to remain statistically significant, with nonoverweight (40th percentile) as the reference group, in multivariate models 1, 3, and 4 (Table 4).
As shown in Tables 4 and 5, the interaction terms for familial risk by familial assets (both developmental periods) were significant in their prediction of membership in the severely obese trajectory. Among children with higher familial assets (both periods), greater exposure to familial risks decreased the odds of membership in the severely obese group relative to the nonoverweight (40th percentile) and overweight/obese trajectories. In other words, higher familial assets buffered the negative effects of familial risks and decreased odds of membership in the severely obese group.
Finally, in Table 4, the interaction term for familial risk by child behavioral regulation (24–54 months) in model 7 predicted membership in the severely obese trajectory such that greater exposure to familial risks (24–54 months) increased the odds of membership in this group (P = .088) only among children with low relative behavioral regulation. No relation was observed among children with high behavioral regulation. This result did not change after adjusting for familial risk (model 8).
Discussion
We took a novel, strength-based approach in identifying the protective effects of early exposure to familial psychosocial assets in infancy/toddlerhood (1–15 months) and early childhood (24–54 months) and child behavioral regulation on BMI growth from 2 to 15 years.36 In our predominantly non-Hispanic, White, English-speaking sample, we found that exposure to familial assets (eg, maternal sensitivity) during early childhood outweighed the effects of exposure to the same set of familial assets during infancy, two sensitive periods of development. Exposure to familial assets in early childhood also reduced the likelihood of obesity and severe obesity, outweighed the positive effects of child behavioral regulation on obesity, and buffered the negative effects of concurrent familial risk on the development of severe obesity. Familial psychosocial assets, such as positive parenting styles and enriched home environments, have been linked to positive developmental outcomes (eg, language development, academic achievement)37–39 and reduced obesity risk, and been shown to facilitate the development of behavioral regulation in childhood,40 even among children who are socioeconomically disadvantaged.41,42 Taken together, our findings highlight the potentially unique and salient role of familial psychosocial assets in promoting obesity resilience.
In the current study, child behavioral regulation reduced the risk for obesity and severe obesity and buffered the negative effects of familial risk among children having an overweight/obese trajectory. Early childhood is a critical period for the development of behavioral regulation processes, such as executive function (a collection of integrated neurocognitive processes involved in higher-order cognition).43,44 Executive function has been highlighted as a potentially modifiable youth-level asset and linked to healthy eating behaviors, less sedentary behavior, greater physical activity,7–9 and obesity resilience in childhood.16,26,45
It is noteworthy that children in the severely obese trajectory had lower family assets and behavioral regulation but did not have greater familial risk exposure compared with nonoverweight children. This finding is consistent with previous work in a socioeconomically disadvantaged sample wherein cumulative psychosocial risk did not distinguish children exhibiting a severely obese trajectory very early in childhood from those with a nonoverweight trajectory, although individual differences were observed in behavioral regulation.45 Other unmeasured genetic and environmental variables may be more potent indicators of risk among children with severe obesity very early in life.
We used a strength-based approach by evaluating the protective role of familial psychosocial assets and child behavioral regulation on BMI growth trajectories. Because familial risk was included in the models, we were able to evaluate the independent and buffering role of family and child-based protective factors on BMI trajectories. A growing number of studies have focused on identifying factors of resilience that help to maintain nonobesity across the lifespan, despite the presence of obesity-related risk factors.24,46–49 Resilience-based frameworks offer a solution-based approach to the study of obesity among at-risk populations50–53 by leveraging assets already present among individuals and families in at-risk settings as well as by highlighting modifiable characteristics of resilient individuals and families.
Study limitations should be noted. A number of risks and assets were not addressed in the current study, including those within the home and neighborhood environments such as parental weight status, youth dietary and physical activity patterns, food insecurity, and social support, all of which may be important indicators of familial and child assets and potential correlates of weight gain during infancy and childhood.54–59 Although the NICHD SECCYD sample largely mirrored the racial/ethnic composition of the United States on the basis of the 1990 census, the sample was predominantly non-Hispanic, White, not poor and excluded non-English speakers. Our findings cannot be generalized to all US families and, in particular, to children from ethnic and socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds. There is evidence to suggest that behavioral regulation may not confer the same degree of protection from obesity among Black children compared with White children.60,61 Also unclear is whether the benefits of familial and child psychosocial assets persist as youth exposure to risk increases. Data collection for this sample occurred between 1991 and 2007, >14 years before this publication. Rates of obesity have increased since then, particularly among children who are ethnic minorities and poor.62 Given the small sample sizes in some BMI trajectories, we were unable to explore differences by sex, race, or ethnicity. Finally, we did not address the stability and chronicity of risk and assets, given that data were not consistently available for all variables of interest.
The American Heart Association recommends that researchers identify resilience factors that buffer the negative effects of childhood adversity on later cardiometabolic outcomes.2 We found in the current study that family psychosocial assets and child behavioral regulation reduce the risk for obesity and severe obesity in youth and buffer the negative effects of early adversity on weight gain. Programs targeting these factors in early childhood have shown promise for obesity prevention.63–69 Early childhood is a sensitive period for the development of self-regulation and positive parenting strategies, and obesity and severe obesity track strongly from childhood into adolescence and adulthood70–72 ; thus, efforts to build strengths in these areas should occur as early in childhood as possible. Pediatricians and other clinicians who monitor children’s growth can use the present findings to tailor messages to parents about the potential value of improving familial and child psychosocial assets for the prevention of childhood obesity, particularly among children exhibiting early signs of an obesity-prone trajectory.
Acknowledgments
The Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD) Early Child Care Research Network was conducted by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development through a cooperative agreement that calls for scientific collaboration between grantees and the NICHD staff (U01 HD019897). The Pennsylvania State University have restricted data-use agreements to analyze the SECCYD data. The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
Dr Rollins conceptualized and designed the study, performed the initial analyses, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Francis conceptualized and designed the study, provided critical oversight for the development of the analytical approach, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Riggs provided insight on the theoretical approach and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.
FUNDING: None.
References
Competing Interests
CONFLICT OF INTEREST DISCLOSURES: The authors have indicated they have no conflicts of interest relevant to this article to disclose.
Comments