Video Abstract

Video Abstract

Close modal
OBJECTIVES

To estimate whether school-based body mass index (BMI) reports impacted the accuracy of children’s self-reported weight category, for children overall and within subgroups.

METHODS

We analyzed existing data from the Fit Study, a randomized controlled trial of a BMI screening and reporting intervention conducted in California from 2014 to 2017. The sample included 4690 children in 27 schools randomized to receive BMI reports and 4975 children in 27 controls schools that received BMI screening only. To estimate how BMI reporting affected accuracy, we fit multinomial logistic regression models to our data. We calculated average marginal effects, which capture the change in probability that children more accurately reported their weight category because of BMI reporting.

RESULTS

We detected no impact of BMI reporting on children’s self-reported weight accuracy. Exploratory subgroup analyses show that for Black children, exposure to 1 round of BMI reporting was associated with a 10.0 percentage point increase in the probability of accurately reporting their weight category (95% confidence interval [CI]: 2.6 to 17.4). Two rounds of reporting were associated with an increase in the probability of accuracy for Asian children (6.6 percentage points; 95% CI: 0.4 to 12.8), 5th graders (11.1 percentage points; 95% CI: 1.6 to 20.5), and those with BMI <5th percentile (17.1 percentage points; 95% CI: 2.7 to 31.6).

CONCLUSIONS

BMI reporting has limited efficacy in increasing children’s weight perception accuracy. Although exploratory analyses show that specific subpopulations became more accurate, future prospective studies should be designed to confirm these results.

What’s Known on This Subject:

Children’s perceptions of their weight have been linked to their exercise and weight control behaviors. Inaccurate perceptions can have both positive and negative consequences in the long run.

What This Study Adds:

We generate new evidence demonstrating that BMI reporting has no effect on the accuracy of children’s weight perceptions. Exploratory evidence shows increased accuracy among Black children, Asian children, 5th graders, and those with BMI <5th percentile.

A complex interplay of factors influences overweight and obesity in children, including children’s biological and psychosocial characteristics, alongside broader contexts within which they develop, such as their family, school, community, and society.1,2  Among these interdependent influences, one potential factor at the individual and psychosocial level is the accuracy of children’s perceived weight. Although some of the strongest predictors predisposing children to overweight and obesity have been traced to children’s parents, including parental BMI,3  how accurately children perceive their weight—especially for those with overweight or obesity—can be a potential precursor to changes in their health behaviors and tentatively linked to healthy weight control behaviors.4,5 

For instance, adolescents who are overweight and accurately perceived their weight are more likely to control their weight by trying to exercise more or eat less.4  Further, adolescents with overweight or obesity are more likely to try to lose weight if they accurately perceived their weight.5  On the other hand, inaccurate perceptions, especially overestimation, can be associated with less than ideal outcomes. Girls who overestimate their weight can experience lower physical and emotional functioning as well as lower parental communication and satisfaction with relationships.6  Weight overestimation can lead to negative outcomes via the internalization of weight-based stigma,6,7  which can result in inappropriate weight control behaviors.6  Finally, in the long run, children with normal weight who inaccurately perceive themselves as overweight face an increased probability of having obesity in young adulthood.7  Yet, inaccurate perceptions can also lead to long run positive consequences, depending on actual weight status and the direction of the misperception. Children with a higher weight status who misperceive themselves as under or normal weight experience lower BMI gains into young adulthood relative to those with accurate perceptions.8  Finally, perceived weight, irrespective of the accuracy, can matter—for girls, perceptions of being overweight regardless of their actual weight status has been linked to lower health-related quality of life.6 

Whereas the accuracy of children’s weight self-perceptions is difficult to change,9  school-based BMI screening and reporting wherein letters containing children’s BMI status alongside messaging about healthy exercise and eating behaviors are sent home to parents10,11  could, in theory, enhance accuracy. Providing BMI reports is a person-based intervention, individualized to each child, and is 1 of many school-based obesity prevention strategies that typically incorporate multipronged components, including altering children’s eating and physical activity environments within schools alongside family- and community-based strategies.12  Although BMI reporting has no effect on reducing children’s BMI,11,13  BMI reports could serve as an informational cue that helps children more accurately align their perceived weight status with their actual weight status. Parents, who play an influential role their children’s health,14  may share BMI information with their children who, in turn, internalize this information, thereby leading them to more accurately self-report their BMI category.

In this follow-up study to the Fit Study, a randomized controlled trial of a BMI screening and reporting intervention targeting children in California,15  we investigate whether BMI reporting influences children’s weight perception accuracy. The original study found no effect on BMI11 ; however, whether weight self-perception accuracy was affected is open to further empirical investigation. Further, we conduct exploratory analyses to examine accuracy within subgroups, by children’s sex, racial and ethnic backgrounds, grade levels, and BMI categories. We examine effects by the social constructs of race and ethnicity, given a recent body of evidence demonstrating that weight perceptions can differ across racial and ethnic groups with varying degrees of accuracy.1618  These differences emerge, in part, from broader structural and societal influences, including socially constructed body image ideals and norms about weight.19  For particular racial and ethnic groups, if these socially constructed ideals and norms produce less than accurate perceptions, then BMI notifications could heighten awareness of actual weight status, consequently leading to enhanced weight perception accuracy.

The Fit Study (fall 2014 to spring 2017) was a cluster randomized clinical trial among California elementary and middle schools that assessed the impact of school-based BMI screening and reporting on student weight status.11,15  Study details, including findings for the primary outcome analyses and trial protocol, are published elsewhere.11  The UC Berkeley Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects approved the Fit Study. We use existing data from the study that included children in 27 treatment schools that were randomly assigned to offer screening and reporting (treatment) and 27 schools randomized to offer screening only (control).

The CONSORT flow diagram for the original cluster randomized trial that we leverage data from for this study is reported elsewhere.11 Figure 1 displays the flow of eligible children with data available for secondary analysis for this study. This eligible sample includes 12 430 children who first entered the study either in fall 2014 or fall 2015 (treatment group: N = 5892; control group: N = 6538). Of this eligible sample, 2765 children (treatment group: n = 1202; control group: n = 1563), approximately 22.2%, were excluded because of missing BMI or child weight perception data. Thus, the final sample used in this study included 9665 children. Children excluded from the analysis were more likely to be Black (12% of excluded students were Black versus 6% of children who were included; P < .001) or white (17% vs 16%; P = .04) and less likely to be Asian (9% vs 16%; P < .001).

FIGURE 1

Flow diagram of eligible students available for secondary data analysis.

FIGURE 1

Flow diagram of eligible students available for secondary data analysis.

Close modal

Children in the final sample were exposed to at least 1 round of BMI screening and reporting (treatment; n = 4690) or screening only (control; n = 4975). Among the 9665 children, 2795 were exposed to 2 rounds of screening and reporting (treatment; n = 1434) or screening only (control; n = 1361). Figure 2 displays a timeline depicting the sample of children who were exposed to at least 1 round and 2 rounds.

FIGURE 2

Intervention timeline and depiction of the study samples. Boxes in gray represent the sample exposed to at least 1 round of screening (control) or screening and reporting (treatment) (n = 9665). The dashed box represents the sample exposed to 2 rounds of screening or screening and reporting (n = 2795).

FIGURE 2

Intervention timeline and depiction of the study samples. Boxes in gray represent the sample exposed to at least 1 round of screening (control) or screening and reporting (treatment) (n = 9665). The dashed box represents the sample exposed to 2 rounds of screening or screening and reporting (n = 2795).

Close modal

BMI

Data on children’s BMI comes from screenings conducted by school staff (PE teachers for 64% of students, nurses [26%], classroom teachers [7%], and office staff or principals or other adults [3%]) in the spring using research grade equipment15 ; staff height and weight measurements were found to be equivalent to those of trained researchers.20  Parents of students in the treatment group were sent a BMI report in the fall, approximately 6 months after BMI was measured in spring. Reports were delivered after 6 months because of the time required to receive, process, and report on the data. A sampleBMI report is included in supplemental materials accompanying previously published findings of the Fit Study.11  Focus groups with diverse groups of California parents informed the reports, which classified children as underweight (BMI <5th percentile); healthy weight (BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile); at risk for overweight (BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile), or overweight (BMI ≥95th percentile for sex and age) per Centers for Disease Control and Prevention standard cut points,21  but adopted an earlier naming convention to avoid labeling children as potentially having “obesity” given parents’ objection to this language.22 

Weight Perception

Data about weight perceptions comes from surveys administered to children in fall, approximately 6 to 9 months after BMI was assessed and 1 to 2 months after parents were sent BMI reports (for treatment students only). Children were asked if they perceived themselves to be very underweight, somewhat underweight, about the right weight, somewhat overweight, or very overweight.23  “Very underweight” and “somewhat underweight” were collapsed to underweight for this analysis.

Weight Perception Accuracy

Using children’s objectively measured BMI from the spring and their self-reported weight perception from the fall, we created a 3-category measure to represent the accuracy of their weight perceptions for each study year. Because the BMI and weight perception accuracy categories used slightly different categorical labels, we aligned the BMI measurement categories to the weight perception categories as follows: underweight(BMI) = very underweight or somewhat underweight (weight perception); healthy weight (BMI) = about the right weight (weight perception); at-risk for overweight (BMI) = somewhat overweight (weight perception); and overweight (BMI) = very overweight (weight perception). Children were classified as (A) “underestimated” if their fall weight perception (eg, underweight) was lower than their subsequent spring BMI category (eg, healthy weight); (B) “accurately estimated” if their fall and spring categories matched each other; or (C) “overestimated” if their fall weight perception (eg, very overweight) was higher than their spring BMI category (eg, at-risk for overweight).

Demographics

Schools provided data on parent-reported sex (male or female), race and ethnicity, and grade. Schools collected race and ethnicity information directly from parents during school enrollment and provided it in categories, including Black, Asian, white, or Hispanic. Children whose parents designated another race or ethnicity, or multiple races, were included in an “all other races and ethnicities” category. We used a “declined to state” category if parents declined to provide race or ethnicity data. Finally, we use school-level data on free and reduced-price meal eligibility from the California Department of Education.24 

To estimate the impact of BMI reporting on students’ weight perception accuracy, we fit the following multinomial logistic regression model for child i in school s as follows:

Where m indexes the number of accuracy categories minus 1 omitted reference category. We used underestimated as the reference category (= 0). The outcome, ln(P(Accuracy Categoryis=m)P(Accuracy Categoryis=0)), is the log odds of being in category m relative to the reference category. γqm captures the effects of the baseline controls xis, whereas β1m is the effect of the treatment on the outcome. Based on results of this model, we calculated the average marginal effect of BMI reporting on the probability that children accurately estimated their weight category. The average marginal effect is estimated by predicting how each child’s probability of being accurate changed if they were in the treatment versus control group, holding all other variables at their observed values, and then averaging over the changes in predicted probabilities. These predicted probabilities are interpreted as the percentage point change in the probability that children accurately reported their weight category. Models included baseline controls for child sex, race and ethnicity, grade level, and school district, as well as school-level proportion of students eligible for free or reduced-price meals. We clustered standard errors by school.

We first fit our model to data on the overall sample. Then, in a series of exploratory analyses, we fit the model stratified by sex, race and ethnicity, grade, and baseline BMI category subgroups. Our analyses by race and ethnicity were based on samples with a reported race or ethnicity; thus, we excluded in those analyses children whose race or ethnicity was unavailable (n = 24). We also fit models separately based on whether the sample received 1 or 2 rounds of BMI reporting. For our overall analysis focusing on all sample children, we adopted a standard level of significance (α = .05) with which to test the null that there was no impact of BMI reporting on the accuracy of children’s weight perceptions. Given the exploratory nature of our subgroup analyses, which typically do not require adjustments for multiple hypothesis testing since the findings are preliminary and used to generate hypotheses rather than confirm them,25  we also adopted a conventional level of significance (α = .05).

For the sample used to estimate the impact of 1 round of BMI reporting (Table 1), slightly more children at baseline were female (51%) and in middle school grades (6th or 7th) (52%). The majority of children were Hispanic (60%) and had a baseline BMI between the 5th and 85th percentile (56%). Similar demographic patterns existed in the analytic sample used to estimate the impact of 2 rounds of BMI reporting. Of note is that the sample used to estimate 2 rounds versus 1 of reporting had a higher proportion of Asian students (29% vs 16%; P < .001) and 6th graders (49% vs 31%; P < .001). Finally, by accuracy category, in the sample of treatment and control schools used to estimate the impact of 1 round of reporting, 48% underestimated, 43% accurately estimated, and 9% overestimated their weight.

TABLE 1

Baseline Characteristics of Children Participating in the Fit Study (Treatment and Control Groups Pooled Together)

Analytic Sample for the Impact of 1 Round of BMI Reporting (n = 9665; Baseline Years: Fall 2014 or Fall 2015)Analytic Sample for the Impact of 2 Rounds of BMI Reporting (n = 2795; Baseline Year: Fall 2014)P for Difference Between Groupsa
Sex, n (%)    
 Female 4935 (51.1) 1379 (49.3) .71 
 Male 4730 (48.9) 1416 (50.7) .71 
Race and ethnicity,bn (%)    
 Black 573 (5.9) 92 (3.3) <.001 
 Asian 1524 (15.8) 800 (28.6) <.001 
 Hispanic 5814 (60.2) 1514 (54.2) <.001 
 White 1517 (15.7) 315 (11.3) <.001 
 All other races and ethnicities 218 (2.2) 69 (2.4) .51 
 Declined to state 19 (0.2) 5 (0.2) .85 
School level, n (%)    
 Elementary, 3rd–5th 4647 (48.1) 1420 (50.8) .01 
 Middle, 6th–8th 5018 (51.9) 1375 (49.2) .01 
Grade level, n (%)    
 4th 3222 (33.3) 953 (34.1) .45 
 5th 1425 (14.7) 467 (16.7) .01 
 6th 2978 (30.8) 1375 (49.2) <.001 
 7th 2040 (21.1) — — 
BMI category, n (%)    
 BMI <5th percentile, underweight 303 (3.1) 129 (4.6) <.001 
 BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile, healthy weight 5430 (56.2) 1609 (57.6) .19 
 BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile, at risk for overweight 1804 (18.7) 524 (18.8) .92 
 BMI ≥95th percentile, overweight 2128 (22.0) 533 (19.1) .001 
Accuracy categories, n (%)    
 Underestimated 4637 (48.0) 1462 (52.3) .007 
 Accurately estimated 4147 (42.9) 1061 (38.0) .002 
 Overestimated 881 (9.1) 272 (9.7) .48 
Analytic Sample for the Impact of 1 Round of BMI Reporting (n = 9665; Baseline Years: Fall 2014 or Fall 2015)Analytic Sample for the Impact of 2 Rounds of BMI Reporting (n = 2795; Baseline Year: Fall 2014)P for Difference Between Groupsa
Sex, n (%)    
 Female 4935 (51.1) 1379 (49.3) .71 
 Male 4730 (48.9) 1416 (50.7) .71 
Race and ethnicity,bn (%)    
 Black 573 (5.9) 92 (3.3) <.001 
 Asian 1524 (15.8) 800 (28.6) <.001 
 Hispanic 5814 (60.2) 1514 (54.2) <.001 
 White 1517 (15.7) 315 (11.3) <.001 
 All other races and ethnicities 218 (2.2) 69 (2.4) .51 
 Declined to state 19 (0.2) 5 (0.2) .85 
School level, n (%)    
 Elementary, 3rd–5th 4647 (48.1) 1420 (50.8) .01 
 Middle, 6th–8th 5018 (51.9) 1375 (49.2) .01 
Grade level, n (%)    
 4th 3222 (33.3) 953 (34.1) .45 
 5th 1425 (14.7) 467 (16.7) .01 
 6th 2978 (30.8) 1375 (49.2) <.001 
 7th 2040 (21.1) — — 
BMI category, n (%)    
 BMI <5th percentile, underweight 303 (3.1) 129 (4.6) <.001 
 BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile, healthy weight 5430 (56.2) 1609 (57.6) .19 
 BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile, at risk for overweight 1804 (18.7) 524 (18.8) .92 
 BMI ≥95th percentile, overweight 2128 (22.0) 533 (19.1) .001 
Accuracy categories, n (%)    
 Underestimated 4637 (48.0) 1462 (52.3) .007 
 Accurately estimated 4147 (42.9) 1061 (38.0) .002 
 Overestimated 881 (9.1) 272 (9.7) .48 

—, Unavailable because 7th graders received only 1 year of BMI reporting.

a

P values derived from unpaired t tests.

b

 Race and ethnicity information comes from schools who collected parental reported information on race and ethnicity at the time of school enrollment. Children of another race or ethnicity or more than 1 race are included in the “All other races and ethnicities” category.

Table 2 displays the differences between children in the treatment and control schools for the samples used to estimate the impact of 1 and 2 rounds of BMI reporting. Among the sample used to estimate the impact of 1 round, children in treatment schools were more likely to be Asian or white, and less likely to be Black or Hispanic, compared with children in control schools (P < .001). Slightly more students in treatment schools were in fourth and slightly fewer were in the 6th grade, compared with children in control schools (P < .001). Among the sample used to estimate the impact of 2 rounds, similar differences also existed, with a few notable exceptions. Children in treatment schools were more likely to be healthy weight or overweight (P = .04), although there was no difference in the proportion of white students and fourth graders between treatment and control groups.

TABLE 2

Baseline Characteristics of Children Participating in the Fit Study by Treatment (BMI Screening and Reporting) and Control (BMI Screening Only) Status

Analytic Sample for the Impact of 1 Round of BMI Reporting (n= 9665; Baseline Years: Fall 2014 or Fall 2015)Analytic Sample for the Impact of 2 Rounds of BMI Reporting (n = 2795; Baseline Year: Fall 2014)
Treatment Group: BMI Screening and Reporting (n = 4690)Control Group: BMI Screening Only (n = 4975)Pa for Difference Between Treatment and ControlTreatment Group: BMI Screening and Reporting (n = 1434)Control Group: BMI Screening Only (n = 1361)Pa for Difference Between Treatment and Control
Sex, n (%)       
 Female 2266 (48.3) 2464 (49.5) .23 700 (48.8) 679 (49.9) .57 
 Male 2424 (51.7) 2511 (50.5) .23 734 (51.2) 682 (50.1) .57 
Race and ethnicity, n (%)       
 Black 147 (3.1) 426 (8.6) <.001 17 (1.2) 75 (5.5) <.001 
 Asian 940 (20.0) 584 (11.7) <.001 525 (36.6) 275 (20.2) <.001 
 Hispanic 2659 (56.7) 3155 (63.4) <.001 692 (48.3) 822 (60.4) <.001 
 White 796 (17.0) 721 (14.5) <.001 152 (10.6) 163 (12.0) .25 
 All other races and ethnicities 134 (2.9) 84 (1.7) <.001 44 (3.0) 25 (1.8) .04 
 Declined to state 14 (0.3) 5 (0.1) .03 4 (0.3) 1 (0.1) .20 
School level, n (%)       
 Elementary, 3rd–5th 2388 (50.9) 2259 (45.4) <.001 768 (53.6) 652 (47.9) .003 
 Middle, 6th–8th 2302 (49.1) 2716 (54.6) <.001 666 (46.4) 709 (52.1) .003 
Grade level, n (%)       
 4th 1675 (35.7) 1547 (31.1) <.001 484 (33.8) 469 (34.5) .69 
 5th 713 (15.2) 712 (14.3) .22 284 (19.8) 183 (13.5) <.001 
 6th 1339 (28.6) 1639 (32.9) <.001 666 (46.4) 709 (52.1) .003 
 7th 963 (20.5) 1077 (21.7) .18 — — — 
BMI Category, n (%)       
 BMI <5th percentile, underweight 169 (3.6) 134 (2.7) .01 74 (5.2) 55 (4.0) .16 
 BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile, healthy weight 2673 (57.0) 2757 (55.4) .12 853 (59.5) 756 (55.6) .04 
 BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile, at risk for overweight 838 (17.9) 966 (19.4) .06 255 (17.8) 269 (19.8) .18 
 BMI ≥95th percentile, overweight 1010 (21.5) 1118 (22.5) .27 252 (17.6) 281 (20.7) .04 
Analytic Sample for the Impact of 1 Round of BMI Reporting (n= 9665; Baseline Years: Fall 2014 or Fall 2015)Analytic Sample for the Impact of 2 Rounds of BMI Reporting (n = 2795; Baseline Year: Fall 2014)
Treatment Group: BMI Screening and Reporting (n = 4690)Control Group: BMI Screening Only (n = 4975)Pa for Difference Between Treatment and ControlTreatment Group: BMI Screening and Reporting (n = 1434)Control Group: BMI Screening Only (n = 1361)Pa for Difference Between Treatment and Control
Sex, n (%)       
 Female 2266 (48.3) 2464 (49.5) .23 700 (48.8) 679 (49.9) .57 
 Male 2424 (51.7) 2511 (50.5) .23 734 (51.2) 682 (50.1) .57 
Race and ethnicity, n (%)       
 Black 147 (3.1) 426 (8.6) <.001 17 (1.2) 75 (5.5) <.001 
 Asian 940 (20.0) 584 (11.7) <.001 525 (36.6) 275 (20.2) <.001 
 Hispanic 2659 (56.7) 3155 (63.4) <.001 692 (48.3) 822 (60.4) <.001 
 White 796 (17.0) 721 (14.5) <.001 152 (10.6) 163 (12.0) .25 
 All other races and ethnicities 134 (2.9) 84 (1.7) <.001 44 (3.0) 25 (1.8) .04 
 Declined to state 14 (0.3) 5 (0.1) .03 4 (0.3) 1 (0.1) .20 
School level, n (%)       
 Elementary, 3rd–5th 2388 (50.9) 2259 (45.4) <.001 768 (53.6) 652 (47.9) .003 
 Middle, 6th–8th 2302 (49.1) 2716 (54.6) <.001 666 (46.4) 709 (52.1) .003 
Grade level, n (%)       
 4th 1675 (35.7) 1547 (31.1) <.001 484 (33.8) 469 (34.5) .69 
 5th 713 (15.2) 712 (14.3) .22 284 (19.8) 183 (13.5) <.001 
 6th 1339 (28.6) 1639 (32.9) <.001 666 (46.4) 709 (52.1) .003 
 7th 963 (20.5) 1077 (21.7) .18 — — — 
BMI Category, n (%)       
 BMI <5th percentile, underweight 169 (3.6) 134 (2.7) .01 74 (5.2) 55 (4.0) .16 
 BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile, healthy weight 2673 (57.0) 2757 (55.4) .12 853 (59.5) 756 (55.6) .04 
 BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile, at risk for overweight 838 (17.9) 966 (19.4) .06 255 (17.8) 269 (19.8) .18 
 BMI ≥95th percentile, overweight 1010 (21.5) 1118 (22.5) .27 252 (17.6) 281 (20.7) .04 

—, Estimates are unavailable because 7th graders received only 1 year of BMI reporting.

a

P values derived from t tests.

We found no impact of BMI reporting on children’s self-reported accuracy in the overall sample after 1 round (average marginal effect [AME]: 0.2; 95% confidence interval [CI]: −1.3 to 1.7, P = .82) or 2 rounds (AME: 3.9; 95% CI: −0.7 to 8.5, P = .09) of BMI reporting. Through our exploratory subgroup analyses, we detected statistically significant associations within subgroups by race, grade levels, and BMI categories. As shown in Table 3, for Black children, exposure to 1 round of BMI reporting was related to a 10.0 percentage point (95% CI: 2.6 to 17.4, P = .008) increase in accuracy. However, there was no association after exposure to 2 consecutive rounds of BMI reporting, each a year apart (AME: 9.4; 95% CI: −9.1 to 27.9, P = .32). On the other hand, for Asian children, exposure to 2 consecutive rounds of BMI reporting was associated with a 6.6 percentage point (95% CI: 0.4 to 12.8, P = .04) increase in accuracy. Finally, 5th graders had a higher probability of accurate weight estimation after 2 rounds of BMI reporting (AME: 11.1; 95% CI: 1.6 to 20.5, P = .02) as well as those with BMI <5th percentile (AME: 17.1; 95% CI: 2.7 to 31.6, P = .02).

TABLE 3

Average Marginal Effects of BMI Screening and Reporting on the Accuracy of Children’s Self-Reported BMI Category, Overall and by Subgroups

After Exposure to 1 Round of BMI ReportingaAfter Exposure to 2 Rounds of BMI Reporting
Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Treatment Group (BMI Screening and Reporting) (95% CI)Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Control Group (BMI Screening only) (95% CI)Average Marginal Effect (Treatment minus Control Difference in Predicted Probabilities) (95% CI)Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Treatment Group (BMI Screening and Reporting) (95% CI)Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Control Group (BMI Screening only) (95% CI)Average Marginal Effect (Treatment minus ControlDifference in Predicted Probabilities) (95% CI)
All students 48.1 (47.2 to 49.0) 47.9 (46.8 to 49.0) 0.2 (−1.3 to 1.7) 54.2 (51.3 to 57.1) 50.3 (47.4 to 53.2) 3.9 (−0.7 to 8.5) 
Sex 
 Female 51.5 (50.1 to 52.9) 51.10 (49.4 to 52.8) 0.4 (−2.0 to 2.8) 57.0 (53.7 to 60.4) 53.7 (50.4 to 58.0) 3.3 (−1.9 to 8.6) 
 Male 44.9 (43.2 to 46.5) 44.7 (43.3 to 46.1) 0.1 (−2.3 to 2.5) 51.2 (48.2 to 54.2) 47.2 (43.2 to 51.1) 4.0 (−1.4 to 9.5) 
Race and ethnicity 
 Black 52.8 (46.9 to 58.7) 42.8 (39.9 to 45.8) 10.0** (2.6 to 17.4) 52.9 (39.8 to 66.1) 43.5 (36.7 to 50.3) 9.4 (−9.1 to 27.9) 
 Asian 53.9 (51.9 to 56.0) 55.4 (52.7 to 58.1) −1.5 (−5.2 to 2.2) 62.4 (58.6 to 66.1) 55.8 (51.9 to 59.6) 6.6* (0.4 to 12.8) 
 Hispanic 45.5 (44.3 to 46.7) 44.8 (43.7 to 46.0) 0.6 (−1.1 to 2.4) 49.4 (46.6 to 52.3) 45.2 (41.5 to 49.0) 4.2 (−1.1 to 9.5) 
 White 51.9 (49.4 to 54.4) 54.0 (50.7 to 57.2) −2.1 (−6.3 to 2.2) 57.2 (50.0 to 64.3) 59.0 (53.1 to 64.9) −1.9 (−12.0 to 8.3) 
Grade level 
 4th 46.2 (44.5 to 47.9) 46.4 (44.8 to 48.1) −0.2 (−2.6 to 2.1) 51.7 (46.4 to 57.2) 51.1 (46.2 to 56.0) 0.7 (−6.9 to 8.3) 
 5th 46.5 (43.5 to 49.5) 45.7 (41.6 to 49.9) 0.7 (−4.3 to 5.9) 58.2 (54.7 to 61.7) 47.1 (38.4 to 55.7) 11.1* (1.6 to 20.5) 
 6th 47.5 (44.5 to 50.6) 50.2 (48.6 to 51.8) −2.6 (−6.7 to 1.5) 54.4 (50.8 to 58.1) 50.1 (47.2 to 53.0) 4.3 (−1.4 to 10.1) 
 7th 51.4 (47.7 to 55.1) 49.4 (45.5 to 53.2) 2.0 (−4.4 to 8.5) — — — 
BMI category 
 BMI <5th percentile, underweight 74.3 (68.8 to 79.8) 65.39 (58.6 to 72.1) 8.9 (−0.5 to 18.1) 88.0 (78.8 to 97.3) 70.9 (62.2 to 79.7) 17.1* (2.7 to 31.6) 
 BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile, healthy wt 64.8 (63.0 to 66.2) 63.3 (61.8 to 64.9) 1.2 (−1.1 to 3.6) 67.8 (64.7 to 70.9) 63.6 (60.5 to 66.7) 4.2 (−0.7 to 9.1) 
 BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile, at risk for overweight 33.6 (30.6 to 36.6) 37.5 (35.2 to 39.8) −3.9 (−7.9 to 0.2) 41.9 (35.8 to 48.0) 42.2 (35.1 to 49.4) −0.4 (−11.1 to 10.4) 
 BMI ≥ 95th percentile, overweight 14.7 (12.3 to 17.2) 14.3 (12.1 to 16.4) 0.5 (−3.0 to 3.9) 16.6 (13.3 to 20.0) 12.9 (10.1 to 15.6) 3.8 (−0.8 to 8.4) 
After Exposure to 1 Round of BMI ReportingaAfter Exposure to 2 Rounds of BMI Reporting
Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Treatment Group (BMI Screening and Reporting) (95% CI)Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Control Group (BMI Screening only) (95% CI)Average Marginal Effect (Treatment minus Control Difference in Predicted Probabilities) (95% CI)Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Treatment Group (BMI Screening and Reporting) (95% CI)Predicted Probability of Accuracy if in Control Group (BMI Screening only) (95% CI)Average Marginal Effect (Treatment minus ControlDifference in Predicted Probabilities) (95% CI)
All students 48.1 (47.2 to 49.0) 47.9 (46.8 to 49.0) 0.2 (−1.3 to 1.7) 54.2 (51.3 to 57.1) 50.3 (47.4 to 53.2) 3.9 (−0.7 to 8.5) 
Sex 
 Female 51.5 (50.1 to 52.9) 51.10 (49.4 to 52.8) 0.4 (−2.0 to 2.8) 57.0 (53.7 to 60.4) 53.7 (50.4 to 58.0) 3.3 (−1.9 to 8.6) 
 Male 44.9 (43.2 to 46.5) 44.7 (43.3 to 46.1) 0.1 (−2.3 to 2.5) 51.2 (48.2 to 54.2) 47.2 (43.2 to 51.1) 4.0 (−1.4 to 9.5) 
Race and ethnicity 
 Black 52.8 (46.9 to 58.7) 42.8 (39.9 to 45.8) 10.0** (2.6 to 17.4) 52.9 (39.8 to 66.1) 43.5 (36.7 to 50.3) 9.4 (−9.1 to 27.9) 
 Asian 53.9 (51.9 to 56.0) 55.4 (52.7 to 58.1) −1.5 (−5.2 to 2.2) 62.4 (58.6 to 66.1) 55.8 (51.9 to 59.6) 6.6* (0.4 to 12.8) 
 Hispanic 45.5 (44.3 to 46.7) 44.8 (43.7 to 46.0) 0.6 (−1.1 to 2.4) 49.4 (46.6 to 52.3) 45.2 (41.5 to 49.0) 4.2 (−1.1 to 9.5) 
 White 51.9 (49.4 to 54.4) 54.0 (50.7 to 57.2) −2.1 (−6.3 to 2.2) 57.2 (50.0 to 64.3) 59.0 (53.1 to 64.9) −1.9 (−12.0 to 8.3) 
Grade level 
 4th 46.2 (44.5 to 47.9) 46.4 (44.8 to 48.1) −0.2 (−2.6 to 2.1) 51.7 (46.4 to 57.2) 51.1 (46.2 to 56.0) 0.7 (−6.9 to 8.3) 
 5th 46.5 (43.5 to 49.5) 45.7 (41.6 to 49.9) 0.7 (−4.3 to 5.9) 58.2 (54.7 to 61.7) 47.1 (38.4 to 55.7) 11.1* (1.6 to 20.5) 
 6th 47.5 (44.5 to 50.6) 50.2 (48.6 to 51.8) −2.6 (−6.7 to 1.5) 54.4 (50.8 to 58.1) 50.1 (47.2 to 53.0) 4.3 (−1.4 to 10.1) 
 7th 51.4 (47.7 to 55.1) 49.4 (45.5 to 53.2) 2.0 (−4.4 to 8.5) — — — 
BMI category 
 BMI <5th percentile, underweight 74.3 (68.8 to 79.8) 65.39 (58.6 to 72.1) 8.9 (−0.5 to 18.1) 88.0 (78.8 to 97.3) 70.9 (62.2 to 79.7) 17.1* (2.7 to 31.6) 
 BMI ≥5th percentile and <85th percentile, healthy wt 64.8 (63.0 to 66.2) 63.3 (61.8 to 64.9) 1.2 (−1.1 to 3.6) 67.8 (64.7 to 70.9) 63.6 (60.5 to 66.7) 4.2 (−0.7 to 9.1) 
 BMI ≥85th percentile and <95th percentile, at risk for overweight 33.6 (30.6 to 36.6) 37.5 (35.2 to 39.8) −3.9 (−7.9 to 0.2) 41.9 (35.8 to 48.0) 42.2 (35.1 to 49.4) −0.4 (−11.1 to 10.4) 
 BMI ≥ 95th percentile, overweight 14.7 (12.3 to 17.2) 14.3 (12.1 to 16.4) 0.5 (−3.0 to 3.9) 16.6 (13.3 to 20.0) 12.9 (10.1 to 15.6) 3.8 (−0.8 to 8.4) 

—, Children were enrolled in the study through the eighth grade; therefore, treatment group students who were in seventh grade at baseline only received 1 year of BMI reporting.

**

P < .01;

*

P < .05.

a

Results based on multinomial logistic regression models that were adjusted, when applicable, for district and student-level factors (sex, race and ethnicity, baseline BMI category, grade) and school-level characteristics (proportion of students who qualify for free or reduced-price meals), accounting for clustering by school.

The accuracy of children’s weight perceptions is 1 factor among a set of complex and interdependent influences at the individual, family, community, and societal levels that has been shown to be linked to children’s weight control behaviors, particularly among children with overweight or obesity.4,5,26  In this follow-up study to the Fit Study, a large-scale randomized controlled trial to evaluate BMI measurement and reporting, we found that BMI reporting did not improve the accuracy of children’s weight perceptions, including those with overweight or obesity. Our exploratory subgroup analyses show that several subgroups became more accurate in their weight self-perceptions: Black children (after exposure to 1 round of BMI reporting) as well as Asian children, fifth graders, and those with BMI <5th percentile (after exposure to 2 rounds of BMI reporting). Given that these subgroup findings are exploratory, future prospective studies that are theoretically grounded can be designed to confirm these results and to also establish whether accuracy influences their weight and weight-related outcomes. Further, studies that focus on specific racial and ethnic groups can assess whether there is an interplay between BMI notification and socially constructed ideals and norms about weight experienced by certain racial and ethnic groups that helps drive specific groups to have greater perception accuracy. Finally, additional effects that could be further examined include whether the direction of the shift in weight perceptions of children in particular weight categories influence dietary practices. For example, analyses can investigate whether underweight children in fifth grade who believe they were normal weight and became more accurate in their weight perceptions adopt less healthy dietary practices to “bulk up.”

Although school-based BMI screening is an important tool for surveillance purposes,27  BMI screening and reporting have been shown to have no impact on BMI11,28  and the present results may help elucidate 1 mechanism for why no effects have been found. In theory, weight perception can serve as a potential antecedent to behavioral changes in diet and exercise; yet, without increasing perception accuracy, BMI reporting may not have changed children’s behaviors sufficiently enough to lead to lower BMI.

Despite evidence that BMI reporting has no effect on either BMI or weight self-perception accuracy, schools are likely to continue to measure children’s BMI and share such information with parents. Given this, the CDC promotes safeguards to ensure that notifications are nonstigmatizing, actionable, and accessible, both in terms of reading level and language.29  Notifications should also be designed in the context of cultural norms and values.30  Lastly, given concerns about widening existing health disparities over the course of the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic, including increases to BMI percentile,3133  schools and districts that already conduct BMI measurement programs may find it valuable to continue collecting height and weight data to better understand how coronavirus disease 2019 disrupted childhood obesity prevalence.

These results are specific to California schools in the Fit Study and may not hold in other state contexts. Additionally, our subgroup findings are exploratory rather than confirmatory and because of multiple hypothesis testing, these findings are subject to an inflated Type I error rate. Also, students were surveyed 6 to 9 months after they participated in BMI assessment, and we assumed that their measured BMI category remained stable between the assessment and survey. Finally, those receiving 1 versus 2 rounds of BMI reporting represent different subsamples that are demographically distinct. Notably, a higher proportion of Asian children received 2 rounds (29%) versus 1 (16%), and a lower proportion of Black and Hispanic children received 2 rounds versus 1 (3% versus 6% and 54% versus 60%, respectively). These differences limit our ability to directly compare the efficacy of 1 versus 2 rounds of BMI reporting within the same group of children.

In this follow-up study to the Fit Study, we found that BMI reporting did not improve the accuracy of children’s perceived weight status, a finding that offers additional insight into why BMI reports sent home to parents, as a behavior change strategy, may have had no impact on children’s BMI. Despite the lack of impact on both BMI and the accuracy of weight self-perceptions, BMI information gathered and reported at the aggregate level can be used to understand patterns and trends in overweight and obesity and to also help target interventions to schools who can most benefit from evidence-based34  multipronged approaches.

We thank reviewers at the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention for comments on previous versions of this manuscript.

Dr Gee conceptualized and designed the study, oversaw the development and execution of the statistical analysis plan, drafted the introduction and results of the study, and developed and revised drafts of the manuscript; Dr Thompson conceptualized and designed the study, conducted the statistical analyses, oversaw the development of the methods section of the study, and developed and revised drafts of the manuscript; Dr Sliwa conceptualized and designed the study, oversaw the development of the study’s discussion section, and developed and revised drafts of the manuscript; Dr Madsen conceptualized and designed the study as well as revised drafts of the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

The data for this study comes from a clinical trial registered with ClinicalTrials.gov, registration number NCT02088086, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02088086. Deidentified individual participant data will not be made available.

The findings and conclusions in this report are those of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official position of the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention.

FUNDING: The data for this study comes from work that was funded by grant R01HD074759 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute of the National Institutes of Health.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST DISCLOSURES: The authors have indicated they have no conflicts of interest relevant to this article to disclose.

1
Kumar
S
,
Kelly
AS
.
Review of childhood obesity: from epidemiology, etiology, and comorbidities to clinical assessment and treatment
.
Mayo Clin Proc
.
2017
;
92
(
2
):
251
265
2
Smith
JD
,
Fu
E
,
Kobayashi
MA
.
Prevention and management of childhood obesity and its psychological and health comorbidities
.
Annu Rev Clin Psychol
.
2020
;
16
:
351
378
3
Vos
MB
,
Welsh
J
.
Childhood obesity: update on predisposing factors and prevention strategies
.
Curr Gastroenterol Rep
.
2010
;
12
(
4
):
280
287
4
Edwards
NM
,
Pettingell
S
,
Borowsky
IW
.
Where perception meets reality: self-perception of weight in overweight adolescents
.
Pediatrics
.
2010
;
125
(
3
):
e452
e458
5
Yang
K
,
Turk
MT
,
Allison
VL
,
James
KA
,
Chasens
E
.
Body mass index self-perception and weight management behaviors during late adolescence
.
J Sch Health
.
2014
;
84
(
10
):
654
660
6
Farhat
T
,
Iannotti
RJ
,
Summersett- Ringgold
F
.
Weight, weight perceptions, and health-related quality of life among a national sample of US girls
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2015
;
36
(
5
):
313
323
7
Sutin
AR
,
Terracciano
A
.
Body weight misperception in adolescence and incident obesity in young adulthood
.
Psychol Sci
.
2015
;
26
(
4
):
507
511
8
Sonneville
KR
,
Thurston
IB
,
Milliren
CE
,
Kamody
RC
,
Gooding
HC
,
Richmond
TK
.
Helpful or harmful? Prospective association between weight misperception and weight gain among overweight and obese adolescents and young adults
.
Int J Obes
.
2016
;
40
(
2
):
328
332
9
Sugiyama
T
,
Horino
M
,
Inoue
K
, %
Kobayashi
Y
,
Shapiro
MF
,
McCarthy
WJ
.
Trends of child’s weight perception by children, parents, and healthcare professionals during the time of terminology change in childhood obesity in the United States, 2005-2014
.
Child Obes
.
2016
;
12
(
6
):
463
473
10
Nihiser
AJ
,
Lee
SM
,
Wechsler
H
, et al
.
Body mass index measurement in schools
.
J Sch Health
.
2007
;
77
(
10
):
651
671
,
quiz 722–724
11
Madsen
KA
,
Thompson
HR
,
Linchey
J
, et al
.
Effect of school-based body mass index reporting in California public schools: a randomized clinical trial
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2021
;
175
(
3
):
251
259
12
Kropski
JA
,
Keckley
PH
,
Jensen
GL
.
School-based obesity prevention programs: an evidence-based review
.
Obesity (Silver Spring)
.
2008
;
16
(
5
):
1009
1018
13
Gee
KA
.
School-based body mass index screening and parental notification in late adolescence: evidence from Arkansas’s act 1220
.
J Adolesc Health
.
2015
;
57
(
3
):
270
276
14
Umberson
D
,
Crosnoe
R
,
Reczek
C
.
Social relationships and health behavior across the life course
.
Annu Rev Sociol
.
2010
;
36
(
1
):
139
157
15
Madsen
KA
,
Linchey
J
,
Ritchie
L
, %
Thompson
HR
.
The Fit Study: design and rationale for a cluster randomized trial of school-based BMI screening and reporting
.
Contemp Clin Trials
.
2017
;
58
:
40
46
16
Duong
HT
,
Roberts
RE
.
Discordance between measured weight, perceived weight, and body satisfaction among adolescents
.
J Psychosom Res
.
2016
;
88
:
22
27
17
Hahn
SL
,
Borton
KA
,
Sonneville
KR
.
Cross-sectional associations between weight-related health behaviors and weight misperception among U.S. adolescents with overweight/obesity
.
BMC Public Health
.
2018
;
18
(
1
):
514
18
Sarafrazi
N
,
Hughes
JP
,
Borrud
L
,
Burt
V
,
Paulose-Ram
R
.
Perception of weight status in U.S. children and adolescents aged 8-15 years, 2005–2012
.
NCHS Data Brief
.
2014
;(
158
):
1
7
19
Langellier
BA
,
Glik
D
,
Ortega
AN
,
Prelip
ML
.
Trends in racial/ethnic disparities in overweight self-perception among US adults, 1988-1994 and 1999-2008
.
Public Health Nutr
.
2015
;
18
(
12
):
2115
2125
20
Thompson
HR
,
Linchey
JK
,
King
B
,
Himes
JH
,
Madsen
KA
.
Accuracy of school staff-measured height and weight used for body mass index screening and reporting
.
J Sch Health
.
2019
;
89
(
8
):
629
635
21
Kuczmarski
RJ
,
Ogden
CL
,
Grummer-Strawn
LM
, et al
.
CDC growth charts: United States
.
Adv Data
.
2000
;(
314
):
1
27
22
Thompson
HR
,
Linchey
JK
,
Madsen
KA
.
Critical elements of a school report to parents on body mass index
.
Prev Chronic Dis
.
2015
;
12
:
E136
23
Neumark-Sztainer
D
,
Paxton
SJ
, %
Hannan
PJ
,
Haines
J
,
Story
M
.
Does body satisfaction matter? Five-year longitudinal associations between body satisfaction and health behaviors in adolescent females and males
.
J Adolesc Health
.
2006
;
39
(
2
):
244
251
24
California Department of Education
.
Public schools and districts data files
.
Available at: https://www.cde.ca.gov/ds/si/ds/pubschls.asp. Accessed October 6, 2020
25
Bender
R
,
Lange
S
.
Adjusting for multiple testing--when and how?
J Clin Epidemiol
.
2001
;
54
(
4
):
343
349
26
Yost
J
,
Krainovich-Miller
B
,
Budin
W
,
Norman
R
.
Assessing weight perception accuracy to promote weight loss among U.S. female adolescents: a secondary analysis
.
BMC Public Health
.
2010
;
10
:
465
27
Patel
AI
,
Sanchez-Vaznaugh
EV
, %
Woodward-Lopez
G
.
The importance of body mass index assessment and surveillance in schools
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2021
;
175
(
6
):
645
28
Almond
D
,
Lee
A
,
Schwartz
AE
.
Impacts of classifying New York City students as overweight
.
Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
.
2016
;
113
(
13
):
3488
3491
29
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Body mass index (BMI) measurement in schools
.
30
Fitzgibbon
ML
,
Beech
BM
.
The role of culture in the context of school-based BMI screening
.
Pediatrics
.
2009
;
124
(
Suppl 1
):
S50
S62
31
Burkart
S
,
Parker
H
,
Weaver
RG
, et al
.
Impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on elementary schoolers’ physical activity, sleep, screen time and diet: a quasi-experimental interrupted time series study
.
Pediatr Obes
.
2022
;
17
(
1
):
e12846
32
Lange
SJ
,
Kompaniyets
L
,
Freedman
DS
, et al;
DNP3
.
Longitudinal trends in body mass index before and during the COVID-19 pandemic among persons aged 2–19 years—United States, 2018–2020
.
MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep
.
2021
;
70
(
37
):
1278
1283
33
Jenssen
BP
,
Kelly
MK
,
Powell
M
, %
Bouchelle
Z
,
Mayne
SL
,
Fiks
AG
.
COVID-19 and changes in child obesity
.
Pediatrics
.
2021
;
147
(
5
):
e2021050123
34
Sliwa
SA
,
Brener
ND
,
Lundeen
EA
,
Lee
SM
.
Do schools that screen for body mass index have recommended safeguards in place?
J Sch Nurs
.
2019
;
35
(
4
):
299
308