Video Abstract

Video Abstract

Close modal
BACKGROUND

Intellectual ability predicts functional outcomes for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). It is essential to classify ASD children with and without intellectual disability (ID) to aid etiological research, provide services, and inform evidence-based educational and health planning.

METHODS

Using a cross-sectional study design, data from 2000 to 2016 active ASD surveillance among 8-year-olds residing in the New York-New Jersey Metropolitan Area were analyzed to determine ASD prevalence with and without ID. Multivariable Poisson regression models were used to identify trends for ASD with ID (ASD-I) and without ID (ASD-N).

RESULTS

Overall, 4661 8-year-olds were identified with ASD. Those that were ASI-I were 1505 (32.3%) and 2764 (59.3%) were ASD-N. Males were 3794 (81.4%), 946 (20.3%) were non-Hispanic Black (Black), 1230 (26.4%) were Hispanic, and 2114 (45.4%) were non-Hispanic white (white). We observed 2-fold and 5-fold increases in the prevalence of ASD-I and ASD-N, respectively, from 2000-2016. Black children were 30% less likely to be identified with ASD-N compared with white children. Children residing in affluent areas were 80% more likely to be identified with ASD-N compared with children in underserved areas. A greater proportion of children with ASD-I resided in vulnerable areas compared with children with ASD-N. Males had higher prevalence compared with females regardless of ID status; however, male-to-female ratios were slightly lower among ASD-I compared with ASD-N cases.

CONCLUSIONS

One-in-3 children with ASD had ID. Disparities in the identification of ASD without ID were observed among Black and Hispanic children as well as among children residing in underserved areas.

What is Known on This Subject:

At present, intellectual ability remains the best predictor of functional outcomes and classification of degree of impairment among children with ASD. Studies have shown that many children with ASD do not have ID.

What This Study Adds:

This study evaluated time trends in ASD with and without ID from 2000 to 2016 from a population-based study in a large and diverse metropolitan area and evaluated trends by sociodemographic factors. Health disparities were identified in ASD identification.

Over the past decades, research has shown multifold increases in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) prevalence in the United States.14  Although the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) estimated ASD prevalence at 0.6% in 2000, by 2018, estimates rose to 2.3% (1-in-44 8-year-old children), surpassing intellectual disability (ID) as 1 of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders among US children.46  Heterogeneity of ASD expression presents a challenge for research on etiology and interventions.7,8  At present, intellectual ability represented by IQ remains the best predictor of functional outcomes and classification of ASD.9,10  Before 2000, estimates suggested up to 75% of ASD children had ID. Recent studies report that 30% to 40% of ASD children have ID, indicating better identification of children with ASD without ID.4,916 

Research shows varying ASD prevalence patterns when intellectual ability is considered. For example, although overall ASD prevalence estimates are considerably higher for males than females, among individuals with ASD and ID, the sex difference is less pronounced.9,11,12,17  Similarly, recent findings indicate that the average lifetime costs to support individuals with ASD varies by intellectual level with estimated lifetime costs of $2.4 million for individuals with ASD and ID versus $1.4 million for ASD individuals without ID.18  Consistent population-based surveillance is needed to track prevalence trends, describe changes in ASD expression, provide data for planning and allocation of resources and inform policy. Moreover, monitoring of ASD trends with consideration of intellectual disability can provide useful information on overall in population health and disparities as well as inform strategies for early ASD identification and intervention.

Few studies have examined trends in ASD prevalence and intellectual ability referencing demographic correlates, such as race and socioeconomic status (SES). Given the evidence demonstrating disparities in health and educational services, better understanding of the prevalence of ASD and ID by sociodemographic factors is needed.19,20  For example, recent analysis showed that Non-Hispanic Black children with ASD were less likely to participate in Early Intervention Programs compared with Non-Hispanic white children.21  Additionally, many studies have documented disparities in early ASD identification among Hispanic and Non-Hispanic Black children.22,23  Similarly, SES is associated with early identification and services.21,24,25  Understanding the associations of ASD and intellectual ability can inform recommendations and interventions provided by pediatricians and to guide future policy regarding ASD.

The current literature on the topic of ASD and ID has many limitations: some provide cross-sectional reports at single time points,12  whereas others rely on administrative or clinical data that could lead to underestimation and/or bias,9,2629  others lack recent data,14,30,31  or use homogeneous samples that are not generalizable to diverse populations.32,33  To address these limitations, we used data from a population-based, active surveillance system to examine intellectual ability among 8-year-old children with ASD in a diverse US metropolitan region, from 2000 to 2016. We describe intellectual ability among children with ASD by sex, race and ethnicity, and SES and characterize temporal trends over this period.

Cross-sectional data from the New Jersey Autism Study, which is part of the CDC - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring (ADDM) Network, were analyzed. ADDM is a population-based active surveillance tracking ASD.34,35  The biannual ADDM surveillance provides ASD prevalence estimates for birth cohorts, at age 8. New Jersey data for 2000, 2002, 2006, 2010, 2012, 2014 and 2016 (representing birth cohorts 1992, 1994, 1998, 2002, 2004, 2006 and 2008, respectively) were included in this analysis.5,13,3640  Data from 2018 surveillance cycle were excluded here because changes in the 2018 ADDM methodology.41 

ADDM ascertainment method is a 2-phase process.35  In phase I, educational and clinical records of children satisfying birth year and residency criteria were reviewed. Records for children showing at least 1 specific, predetermined, ASD indicator were abstracted.35  In phase II, using standard procedures, expert clinician reviewers identified and characterized ASD cases. ASD case definition was satisfied if behaviors documented in professional evaluations reflected the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM)-IV-TR ASD criteria (Supplemental Fig 3). Beginning with the 2014 ASD surveillance cycle, the ADDM Network also included a case definition based on DSM-5 criteria in addition to the case definition based on DSM-IV-TR criteria used from 2000 to 2012. Since ADDM network identified minimal differences in ASD prevalence using DSM-IV-TR and DSM-5 criteria,38  to remain consistent across the study period, the DSM-IV-TR surveillance case definition was used in this analysis.

Across all surveillance cycles, the methodology, ASD case definition and surveillance region remained constant. The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Rutgers University – New Jersey Medical School.

The surveillance region included 4 counties (Essex, Hudson, Ocean, and Union) representing approximately 25% of the total New Jersey (8-year-old) population. The region is within the New York-New Jersey Metropolitan Area, the most populous metropolis and encompasses a diverse population (approximately, 40% to 42% Non-Hispanic white, 22% to 27% Non-Hispanic Black and 25% to 32% Hispanic). Each surveillance cycle included approximately 30 000 children. Population denominators were obtained from the National Center for Health Statistics vintage 2019 bridged-race postcensal data42  (Supplemental Table 4).

The study included 2 case definitions: ASD-I and ASD-N. ASD-I included children with ID as defined by an intelligence quotient (IQ) score ≤ 70. ASD-N was defined as IQ score >70 and included children with borderline, average, and above average IQ. By convention and for research purposes, ID is traditionally defined according to IQ test scores.4,15,26,38  Case definitions reflected the most recent IQ score in each child’s record. Across all cycles, 4661 children were identified with ASD; 81% (n = 3762) had documented IQ scores.

Records of children with missing IQ test data (n = 899) were reviewed to classify children as ASD-I or ASD-N. When IQ test findings were unavailable, ASD-I was determined based on: (1) documented cognitive delay, deficit, or impairment by a professional (n = 181), (2) attempted administration of an IQ test that was discontinued because of nontestable status (n = 117), or (3) a special education classification of cognitive impairment (n = 5) at school. Conversely, ASD-N was determined for children without IQ scores based on: (1) special education classification of speech-only deficit (n = 9), (2) documentation of age-appropriate cognitive skills and/or average or higher academic skills by standardized performance tests (n = 95), or (3) ASD cases classified by the surveillance with mild impairment and no indication of special education services (n = 91). Following this enhanced classification, 398 ASD cases (9%) had undetermined intellectual ability and were excluded from further analysis.

Sex and race and ethnicity data were obtained from individual records and supplemented from birth certificates. Race and ethnicity was categorized for Non-Hispanic white (white), Non-Hispanic Black (Black) and Hispanic. Other race and ethnicity categories were excluded from race and ethnicity analysis because of small sample sizes, and less than 1% (n = 29) of the study population had no race and ethnicity information and were also excluded from race and ethnicity analysis. SES was based on median household income (MHI) at the census tract level and categorized as a 3-level variable based on MHI tertiles for all census tracts in New Jersey, namely: low-SES (MHI ≤ $57 933), mid-SES (MHI = $57 934–$87 313) and high-SES (MHI > $87 313).

Additional SES indicators were considered, including the social vulnerability index (SVI) and the poverty rate, to assess whether different aspects of wealth were also associated with ASD-I and ASD-N. SVI is a CDC-developed multifactorial index representing 15 demographic factors grouped into 4 themes. The overall index (scaled 0–1, with 1 representing highest vulnerability) combines all 4 themes: (1) traditional SES factors; (2) household composition and disability; (3) minority status and language; (4) housing type and transportation. SVI was categorized as a 3-level variable based on all census tracts in New Jersey. Geographic areas with 20% or greater poverty rates were classified as “poverty areas” and areas with less than 20% poverty rates as “nonpoverty areas” consistent with US Census classification.43 

ASD prevalence was estimated overall and by intellectual ability status (ASD-I and ASD-N) in each of the 7 cycles (2000–2016). Wilson score method was used to compute 95% confidence intervals (CI). ASD prevalence was evaluated by sex, race and ethnicity, and SES. Descriptive statistics and Pearson χ2 tests were used to characterize differences between children with ASD-I and ASD-N. Prevalence ratio (PR) and 95% CI were calculated to compare prevalence estimates overall and by sex and race and ethnicity from 2000 to 2016. Analyses examining SES were restricted to surveillance cycles 2010 through 2016 as comparable SES data from 2000 to 2006 were not available. Multivariable Poisson regression was used to analyze trends in ASD rates over time allowing for overdispersion; the log of the population was treated as an offset. Models were stratified by ASD type (ASD-I or ASD-N) and were adjusted for sex, race and ethnicity, SES, and birth year across 2010 to 2016 surveillance cycles. In sensitivity analyses, we refit models employing the stricter definition of ASD-I and ASD-N, based on IQ scores only. Statistical analyses were performed using SAS 9.4.

From 2000 to 2016, 4661 children satisfied the ASD case definition. ASD prevalence estimates increased 3-fold from 9.6 per 1000 (95%CI: 8.5–10.7) in 2000 to 31.8 per 1000 (95%CI: 30.0–33.8) in 2016. During the same period, ASD-I increased 2-fold, from 2.9 per 1000 (95%CI: 3.6–5.0) to 7.3 per 1000 (95%CI: 7.6–9.6), whereas ASD-N increased approximately 5-fold, from 3.8 per 1000 (95%CI: 3.3–4.7) in 2000 to 18.9 per 1000 (95% CI: 19.2–22.3) in 2016 (Fig 1 and Table 1).

FIGURE 1

ASD prevalence estimates per 1000 8-year-old children in New Jersey overall and by intellectual ability.

FIGURE 1

ASD prevalence estimates per 1000 8-year-old children in New Jersey overall and by intellectual ability.

Close modal
TABLE 1

ASD With ID (ASD-I) and ASD Without ID (ASD-N) Prevalence Ratio in New Jersey Overall and by Sex, Race and Ethnicity, 2000 to 2016 and SES 2010 to 2016

Prevalence Ratio (95% CI)
ASD-I, 2016–2000ASD-N, 2016–2000
Overall 2.0 (1.6–2.5) 5.3 (4.4–6.4) 
Sex   
 Male 2.1 (1.6–2.7) 5.2 (4.2–6.4) 
 Female 1.8 (1.2–2.8) 5.8 (3.5–9.3) 
Race and ethnicity   
 Non-Hispanic white 1.5 (1.0–2.2) 4.2 (3.3–5.4) 
 Non-Hispanic Black 2.0 (1.4–2.8) 5.0 (3.1–8.1) 
 Hispanic 2.8 (1.8–4.3) 9.3 (5.6–15.4) 
SES (MHI)a   
 Low SES 1.4 (1.1–1.7) 1.9 (1.5–2.3) 
 Mid SES 1.4 (1.0–2.0) 1.6 (1.3–2.0) 
 High SES 0.7 (0.5–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 
Prevalence Ratio (95% CI)
ASD-I, 2016–2000ASD-N, 2016–2000
Overall 2.0 (1.6–2.5) 5.3 (4.4–6.4) 
Sex   
 Male 2.1 (1.6–2.7) 5.2 (4.2–6.4) 
 Female 1.8 (1.2–2.8) 5.8 (3.5–9.3) 
Race and ethnicity   
 Non-Hispanic white 1.5 (1.0–2.2) 4.2 (3.3–5.4) 
 Non-Hispanic Black 2.0 (1.4–2.8) 5.0 (3.1–8.1) 
 Hispanic 2.8 (1.8–4.3) 9.3 (5.6–15.4) 
SES (MHI)a   
 Low SES 1.4 (1.1–1.7) 1.9 (1.5–2.3) 
 Mid SES 1.4 (1.0–2.0) 1.6 (1.3–2.0) 
 High SES 0.7 (0.5–1.2) 1.0 (0.8–1.3) 

ASD-I, ASD and intellectual disability; ASD-N, ASD without intellectual disability; CI, confidence interval; ID, intellectual disability; MHI, median household income; SES, socioeconomic status.

a

Prevalence ratio compares prevalence estimates between 2016 to 2000 except for SES category comparing 2016 to 2010 for ASD-I and ASD-N cases.

The majority of ASD cases (n = 2764; 59%) satisfied the definition of ASD-N, whereas 32% (n = 1505) met the definition of ASD-I. Among ASD-N cases with IQ scores, 33% (n = 879) had borderline IQ (71–84). ASD-N represented 57% of ASD cases in 2000 as compared with 72% in 2016.

Males had higher overall ASD prevalence estimates compared with females across all birth cohorts, ranging from 3.6 to 4.1 male-to-female ratio across the 16-year period. Prevalence was higher among males regardless of ID status (Figs 2, A and D); however, the male-to-female ratio was higher among ASD-N, compared with ASD-I cases. ASD-N prevalence estimates for males increased 5.2-fold (PR = 5.2; 95% CI: 4.2–6.4), whereas estimates for ASD-I males increased 2.1-fold (PR = 2.1; 95% CI: 1.6–2.5). Similar increases were observed among females with ASD-N (PR = 5.8; 95% CI: 3.5–9.3) and ASD-I (PR = 1.8; 95% CI: 1.2–2.8).

FIGURE 2

ASD-I and ASD-N prevalence per 1000. (A) ASD-I 2000–2016 prevalence by sex. (B) ASD-I 2000–2016 prevalence by race and ethnicity. (C) ASD-I 2010–2016 prevalence by socioeconomic status. (D) ASD-N 2000–2016 prevalence by sex. (E) ASD-N 2000–2016 prevalence by race and ethnicity. (F) ASD-N 2010–2016 prevalence by socioeconomic status.

FIGURE 2

ASD-I and ASD-N prevalence per 1000. (A) ASD-I 2000–2016 prevalence by sex. (B) ASD-I 2000–2016 prevalence by race and ethnicity. (C) ASD-I 2010–2016 prevalence by socioeconomic status. (D) ASD-N 2000–2016 prevalence by sex. (E) ASD-N 2000–2016 prevalence by race and ethnicity. (F) ASD-N 2010–2016 prevalence by socioeconomic status.

Close modal

ASD-I and ASD-N estimates varied by race and ethnicity. Increases in ASD-I and ASD-N were evident across all races and ethnicities between 2000 and 2016, with the greatest increase observed among Black (PR = 5.0; 95% CI: 3.1–8.1) and Hispanic (PR = 9.3; 95% CI: 5.6–15.4) children with ASD-N. By 2016, across all races and ethnicities, ASD-N estimates were higher than ASD-I estimates (Figs 2, B and E).

ASD-I and ASD-N estimates varied by SES. ASD-N prevalence estimates were lower among children residing in low-SES compared with high-SES areas (Fig 2F). From 2010 to 2016, estimates for ASD-N were stable for children residing in High-SES areas but increased 1.9-fold (95% CI: 1.5–2.3) and 1.6-fold (95% CI: 1.3–1.7) among children residing in low and mid-SES areas, respectively. Estimates for ASD-I also increased significantly for children residing in low-SES areas (Table 1). Results using alternative SES metrics were similar. Among ASD-I children, 31% resided in poverty areas, compared with 15% of ASD-N children. Similarly, 68% of ASD-I children resided in SVI-designated highly-vulnerable areas, compared with 38% of ASD-N children (Table 2).

TABLE 2

Sociodemographic Characteristics of Children With ASD With ID (ASD-I) and ASD Without ID (ASD-N)

ASD-IASD-NP
n%n%
Overall 2560  1196   
Sex     .001 
 Male 944 79 2120 83  
 Female 242 21 440 17  
Race and ethnicity     <.001 
 Non-Hispanic white 329 28 1367 53  
 Non-Hispanic Black 387 32 364 14  
 Hispanic 373 31 639 25  
SES (MHI)a     <.001 
 Low SES 770 64 956 37  
 Mid SES 274 23 875 34  
 High SES 152 13 729 29  
SES (poverty)b     <.001 
 Poverty area 325 31 331 15  
 Non-poverty area 732 69 1861 85  
SES (SVI)c      
 High vulnerability 720 68 835 38  
 Mid vulnerability 191 18 678 31  
 Low vulnerability 147 14 680 31  
ASD-IASD-NP
n%n%
Overall 2560  1196   
Sex     .001 
 Male 944 79 2120 83  
 Female 242 21 440 17  
Race and ethnicity     <.001 
 Non-Hispanic white 329 28 1367 53  
 Non-Hispanic Black 387 32 364 14  
 Hispanic 373 31 639 25  
SES (MHI)a     <.001 
 Low SES 770 64 956 37  
 Mid SES 274 23 875 34  
 High SES 152 13 729 29  
SES (poverty)b     <.001 
 Poverty area 325 31 331 15  
 Non-poverty area 732 69 1861 85  
SES (SVI)c      
 High vulnerability 720 68 835 38  
 Mid vulnerability 191 18 678 31  
 Low vulnerability 147 14 680 31  

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ASD-I, ASD and intellectual disability; ASD-N, ASD without intellectual disability; ID, intellectual disability; MHI, median household income; SES, socioeconomic status; SVI, Social Vulnerability Scale.

a

Socioeconomic status based on median household income at the census tract level.

b

Socioeconomic status based on poverty rate at the census tract level.

c

Socioeconomic status based on social vulnerability index.

In multivariable regression analyses, ASD-N was twice as prevalent as ASD-I (adjusted rate ratio [ARR] = 2.1, 95% CI:1.8–2.5). The male-to-female ratio (MF-ARR) was higher among individuals with ASD-N (MF-ARR = 4.4, 95% CI:3.8–5.3) than ASD-I cases (MF-ARR = 3.9, 95% CI:3.3–4.7). Differences by race and ethnicity were evident. Whereas Black (ARR = 2.1, 95% CI:1.7–2.5) and Hispanic (ARR = 1.7, 95% CI:1.4–2.1) children were more likely to be identified with ASD-I, compared with white children, Black children were 30% less likely to be identified with ASD-N compared with white children. Among ASD-I cases there were no differences by SES; however, children residing in high-SES (ARR = 1.8, 95% CI:1.5–2.2) and mid-SES (ARR = 2.0, 95% CI:1.7–2.3) areas were more likely to have ASD-N compared with children from low-SES areas. An increase in identification of ASD-N by birth cohort was observed. Children born in 2008 were 80% more likely to be identified with ASD-N compared with children born in 2002. Similarly, there was a 40% increase in ASD-I identification over time. Sensitivity analyses using a narrower case definition of ASD-I and ASD-N, based solely on IQ scores, yielded similar results (Table 3).

TABLE 3

Multivariable Poisson Regression Model Stratified by ASD Type (ASD-I or ASD-N) and Adjusted for Sociodemographic Factors, 2010 to 2016 and Sensitivity Analysis Using the Narrower ASD Type Case Definition Based on IQ Scores

Based on Enhanced ASD-I and ASD-N Study Case Definition (n = 4263)Based on IQ Scores ASD-I and ASD-N Study Case Definition (n = 3762)
Model 1aModel 2bModel 3cModel 4d
ASD type ASD-I ASD-N ASD-I ASD-N 
Sex     
 Female Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 Male 3.9 (3.3–4.7) 4.4 (3.8–5.3) 3.8 (3.2–4.6) 4.5 (3.8–5.4) 
Race and ethnicity     
 White Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 Black 2.1 (1.7–2.5) 0.7 (0.5–0.8) 2.3 (1.9–2.9) 0.7 (0.6–0.9) 
 Hispanic 1.7 (1.4–2.1) 0.8 (0.7–1.0) 1.8 (1.5–2.2) 0.9 (0.8–1.1) 
SES     
 Low Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 Mid 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 2.0 (1.7–2.3) 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 2.0 (1.7–2.3) 
 High 0.8 (0.6–1.0) 1.8 (1.5–2.2) 0.8 (0.6–1.0) 1.9 (1.6–2.3) 
Birth year     
 2002 Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 2004 1.3 (1.0–1.5) 1.2 (1.0–1.5) 1.1 (0.9–1.4) 1.1 (0.9–1.4) 
 2006 1.5 (1.2–1.8) 1.6 (1.4–2.0) 1.6 (1.3–2.0) 1.6 (1.3–1.9) 
 2008 1.4 (1.1v1.7) 1.8 (1.5–2.2) 1.5 (1.2–1.9) 1.7 (1.4–2.1) 
Based on Enhanced ASD-I and ASD-N Study Case Definition (n = 4263)Based on IQ Scores ASD-I and ASD-N Study Case Definition (n = 3762)
Model 1aModel 2bModel 3cModel 4d
ASD type ASD-I ASD-N ASD-I ASD-N 
Sex     
 Female Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 Male 3.9 (3.3–4.7) 4.4 (3.8–5.3) 3.8 (3.2–4.6) 4.5 (3.8–5.4) 
Race and ethnicity     
 White Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 Black 2.1 (1.7–2.5) 0.7 (0.5–0.8) 2.3 (1.9–2.9) 0.7 (0.6–0.9) 
 Hispanic 1.7 (1.4–2.1) 0.8 (0.7–1.0) 1.8 (1.5–2.2) 0.9 (0.8–1.1) 
SES     
 Low Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 Mid 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 2.0 (1.7–2.3) 1.0 (0.9–1.2) 2.0 (1.7–2.3) 
 High 0.8 (0.6–1.0) 1.8 (1.5–2.2) 0.8 (0.6–1.0) 1.9 (1.6–2.3) 
Birth year     
 2002 Reference Reference Reference Reference 
 2004 1.3 (1.0–1.5) 1.2 (1.0–1.5) 1.1 (0.9–1.4) 1.1 (0.9–1.4) 
 2006 1.5 (1.2–1.8) 1.6 (1.4–2.0) 1.6 (1.3–2.0) 1.6 (1.3–1.9) 
 2008 1.4 (1.1v1.7) 1.8 (1.5–2.2) 1.5 (1.2–1.9) 1.7 (1.4–2.1) 

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ASD-I, ASD and intellectual disability; ASD-N, ASD and no intellectual disability; IQ, intelligence quotient; SES, socioeconomic status.

a

Model 1 evaluated rate ratio for ASD-I (enhanced study case definition) adjusted for sociodemographic factors, from 2010 to 2016 surveillance cycles.

b

Model 2 evaluated rate ratio for ASD-N (enhanced study case definition) adjusted for sociodemographic factors, from 2010 to 2016 surveillance cycles.

c

Model 3 (sensitivity analysis) evaluated rate ratio for ASD-I (IQ scores only) adjusted for sociodemographic factors, from 2010 to 2016 surveillance cycles.

d

Model 4 (sensitivity analysis) evaluated rate ratio for ASD-N (IQ scores only) adjusted for sociodemographic factors, from 2010 to 2016 surveillance cycles.

From 2000 to 2016, we observed a 500% increase in the prevalence of ASD without ID and a 200% increase in the prevalence of ASD with ID using consistent, population-based active surveillance in a diverse and populous region. ASD prevalence increased across all sex, race and ethnicity, and SES subgroups and the greatest increases were seen among children without intellectual impairment. These findings are consistent with prior studies.4,1214  Although earlier studies reported that a large proportion of children with ASD had ID, more recent findings suggest the reverse, namely that most children with ASD have intellectual ability in the nondisabled range. In this study, 57% of ASD cases did not have ID in 2000 versus 72% in 2016, a trend likely explained by better recognition of ASD among children with average intellectual ability. Adjusting for various factors and time trends, for every child identified with ASD-I, 2 children with ASD-N were identified.

Consistent with previous findings, the male-to-female ratio was 3.9 among children with ASD-I; slightly lower than the male-to-female ratio of 4.4 among children with ASD-N. We anticipated a considerably lower sex ratio in the ASD-I group based on recent research.44  The high sex ratio has been consistently observed in ASD populations and has been attributed to possible clinical differences between males and females and under identification and/or ascertainment bias in identifying females with ASD. This analysis shows possible under-identification of females with ASD regardless of ID status as the sex ratio among ASD-I children is 4:1.

By 2016, ASD-N was higher than ASD-I among all races, which suggests better identification and increased identification of children with ASD-N. However, our findings underscore the persistent sociodemographic disparities in the identification of ASD-N. Black children had lower estimates of ASD-N compared with white children, suggesting likely under-identification or misdiagnosis of Black children. Multiple studies have reported racial disparities in ASD identification,23,4548  disparities that may be driven by under-identification or misdiagnosis of children with ASD-N.49  Under-identification may result in loss of access to services. Universal ASD screening at routine pediatric visits is needed to better identify children with moderate to mild forms of ASD. Pediatricians are in an ideal position to address diagnostic inequalities. By 36-months, a child has typically seen a pediatrician at multiple well-child visits, and use of effective screeners to monitor child development can lead to earlier identification and the initiation of appropriate services at earlier ages. Cultural barriers may also impact ASD identification and targeted, culturally-sensitive parent-focused education may increase ASD knowledge and awareness as well as community acceptance, potentially reducing disparities in the utilization of services.5052 

Our findings demonstrate significant health disparities in ASD, particularly in relation to SES. From 2010 to 2016, ASD-N prevalence was higher in affluent areas compared with disadvantaged areas, indicating probable under-identification of children with ASD-N in disadvantaged areas. Many US studies have shown a positive SES gradient in relation to ASD prevalence. This SES relationship remains when intellectual ability is considered. Additionally, our findings indicate that a greater proportion of children with ASD-I reside in underserved, highly vulnerable communities, where they likely have diminished access to care and services. The SES- based differential among ASD-I children shows the need for additional effort to improve the early detection and linkage to services, especially in socially disadvantaged communities.

As identification of ASD-N improves, particularly among Black and Hispanic children, continued increases in ASD prevalence are likely. With up to 72% of the ASD population having borderline or average intellectual ability, emphasis should be placed on early screening, early identification, and early intervention to promote optimal functional outcomes. Studies have shown improved outcomes with early intense interventions5356  and this may be particularly true in ASD children without ID.57  Moreover, research has shown gains in intellectual and adaptive functioning with intense intervention at younger ages in general.53 

This was a population-based study using a standard ASD case definition, in a well-defined, diverse region that used an active case-finding methodology, independent of ASD diagnosis. ASD case definition, region, and methodology were consistent across all surveillance cycles. IQ data were available for 81% of cases and upon further review we determined intellectual ability for 91% of cases. Importantly, the study population was highly diverse and included large numbers of Hispanic and Black children, allowing us to generate accurate estimates for these understudied groups. Although many studies have reported on ASD and ID at a single time point, we examined patterns in ASD prevalence over a 16-year period. In addition, New Jersey is an autism epicenter and may be an indicator of future ASD trends in the United States.

Several limitations are acknowledged, including the broad categorization of ASD into 2 types, ASD-I and ASD-N. A substantial proportion of ASD-N cases had borderline IQ and these children likely experience significant challenges. Additionally, the age of IQ testing was not included in the analysis and ID was defined based on IQ scores without consideration to adaptive scores. Although the surveillance region included 4 urban and suburban counties encompassing a large diverse metropolitan population, the observed findings may not be representative of ASD prevalence across the United States. The ADDM surveillance method uses information from health and educational records, and ascertainment bias cannot be ruled out as some children lacked documented IQ scores.

We observed that 2-in-3 children with ASD do not have cooccurring ID, indicating increased identification of ASD without ID among all demographic subgroups from 2000 to 2016. However, our findings underscore the likely presence of health disparities in ASD without ID identification, especially among disadvantaged children. ASD is a major public health concern and prevalence estimates are likely to continue to rise as disparities are reduced and ASD identification is improved. Since ASD is a complex heterogeneous disorder, it is important to further study ASD in relation to intellectual ability to understand etiology and to inform effective interventions and appropriate services as well as aid in educational and health planning at the community level, as the needs for children with ASD and ID differ from the needs of children with ASD without ID. Furthermore, tracking ASD trends from diverse populations can identify health disparities and provide vital information on shifts in community health over time. Future work should focus on addressing health disparities in the identification of ASD through the expansion of screening programs and improved linkage to care.

This study was made possible by support from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (1U53DD001172) and National Institutes of Health-National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences P30 ES005022. We thank Audrey Mars MD, Mildred Waale LDTC, Arline Fusco PsyD, Tara Gleeson NP, Gail Burack PhD, Paul Zumoff PhD, Rita Baltus, MD, Cindy Cruz, and Michael Verile, as well as the cooperative support and participation of the New Jersey Department of Health and Education and the many school districts and health centers in our region.

Dr Shenouda conceptualized and designed the study, carried out the initial analyses, drafted the initial manuscript, and reviewed the revised manuscript; Dr Barrett conceptualized and designed the study, reviewed the study analyses, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Drs Davidow, and Silenzio reviewed the study analyses, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; Dr Halperin conceptualized and designed the study; Ms Sidwell and Ms Lescott critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Zahorodny conceptualized and designed the study, supervised data collection, and critically reviewed the manuscript for important intellectual content; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

COMPANION PAPER: A companion to this article can be found at http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2022-059541.

FUNDING: All phases of this study were supported by the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention grant NU53DD001172 and National Institutes of Health-National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences grant P30 ES005022.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST DISCLOSURES: The authors have indicated they have no conflicts of interest to disclose.

ADDM

Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network

ASD

Autism Spectrum Disorder

CDC

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention

CI

confidence interval

ID

intellectual disability

MHI

median household income

PR

prevalence ratio

SES

socioeconomic status

1
Chiarotti
F
,
Venerosi
A
.
Epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders: a review of worldwide prevalence estimates since 2014
.
Brain Sci
.
2020
;
10
(
5
):
274
2
Davidovitch
M
,
Hemo
B
,
Manning-Courtney
P
,
Fombonne
E
.
Prevalence and incidence of autism spectrum disorder in an Israeli population
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2013
;
43
(
4
):
785
793
3
Fombonne
E
.
Editorial: the rising prevalence of autism
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2018
;
59
(
7
):
717
720
4
Maenner
MJ
,
Shaw
KA
,
Bakian
AV
, et al
.
Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2018
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2021
;
70
(
11
):
1
16
5
Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2000 Principal Investigators
;
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders--autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, six sites, United States, 2000
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2007
;
56
(
1
):
1
11
6
Patrick
ME
,
Shaw
KA
,
Dietz
PM
, et al
.
Prevalence of intellectual disability among eight-year-old children from selected communities in the United States, 2014
.
Disabil Health J
.
2021
;
14
(
2
):
101023
7
Amaral
DG
,
Anderson
GM
,
Bailey
A
, et al
.
Gaps in current autism research: the thoughts of the Autism Research editorial board and associate editors
.
Autism Res
.
2019
;
12
(
5
):
700
714
8
Russell
G
,
Mandy
W
,
Elliott
D
,
White
R
,
Pittwood
T
,
Ford
T
.
Selection bias on intellectual ability in autism research: a cross-sectional review and meta-analysis
.
Mol Autism
.
2019
;
10
:
9
9
Duvall
SW
,
Huang-Storms
L
,
Presmanes Hill
A
,
Myers
J
,
Fombonne
E
.
No sex differences in cognitive ability in young children with autism spectrum disorder
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2020
;
50
(
5
):
1770
1785
10
Newschaffer
CJ
,
Croen
LA
,
Daniels
J
, et al
.
The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders
.
Annu Rev Public Health
.
2007
;
28
:
235
258
11
Fombonne
E
.
Epidemiological trends in rates of autism
.
Mol Psychiatry
.
2002
;
7
(
Suppl 2
):
S4
S6
12
Katusic
MZ
,
Myers
SM
,
Weaver
AL
,
Voigt
RG
.
IQ in autism spectrum disorder: a population-based birth cohort study
.
Pediatrics
.
2021
;
148
(
6
):
e2020049899
13
Maenner
MJ
,
Shaw
KA
,
Baio
J
, et al;
EdS1
;
PhD-7
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2016
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2020
;
69
(
4
):
1
12
14
Van Naarden Braun
K
,
Christensen
D
,
Doernberg
N
, et al
.
Trends in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder, cerebral palsy, hearing loss, intellectual disability, and vision impairment, Metropolitan Atlanta, 1991-2010
.
PLoS One
.
2015
;
10
(
4
):
e0124120
15
Srivastava
AK
,
Schwartz
CE
.
Intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorders: causal genes and molecular mechanisms
.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev
.
2014
;
46
(
Pt 2
):
161
174
16
Blumberg
SJ
,
Bramlett
MD
,
Kogan
MD
,
Schieve
LA
,
Jones
JR
,
Lu
MC
.
Changes in prevalence of parent-reported autism spectrum disorder in school-aged U.S. children: 2007 to 2011-2012
.
Natl Health Stat Report
.
2013
(
65
):
1
11
,
11
p following 11
.
17
Lord
C
,
Brugha
TS
,
Charman
T
, et al
.
Autism spectrum disorder
.
Nat Rev Dis Primers
.
2020
;
6
(
1
):
5
18
Buescher
AV
,
Cidav
Z
,
Knapp
M
,
Mandell
DS
.
Costs of autism spectrum disorders in the United Kingdom and the United States
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2014
;
168
(
8
):
721
728
19
Aylward
BS
,
Gal-Szabo
DE
,
Taraman
S
.
Racial, ethnic, and sociodemographic disparities in diagnosis of children with autism spectrum disorder
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2021
;
42
(
8
):
682
689
20
Benevides
TW
,
Carretta
HJ
,
Rust
G
,
Shea
L
.
Racial and ethnic disparities in benefits eligibility and spending among adults on the autism spectrum: a cohort study using the Medicare Medicaid Linked Enrollees Analytic Data Source
.
PLoS One
.
2021
;
16
(
5
):
e0251353
21
Shenouda
J
,
Barrett
E
,
Davidow
AL
, et al
.
Disparities in early intervention program participation by children with autism spectrum disorder in a US Metropolitan Area: 2006-2016
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2022
;
176
(
9
):
906
914
22
Shenouda
J
,
Barrett
E
,
Davidow
AL
, %
Halperin
W
,
Silenzio
VMB
,
Zahorodny
W
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in a large, diverse metropolitan area: variation by sociodemographic factors
.
Autism Res
.
2022
;
15
(
1
):
146
155
23
Wiggins
LD
,
Durkin
M
,
Esler
A
, et al
.
Disparities in documented diagnoses of autism spectrum disorder based on demographic, individual, and service factors
.
Autism Res
.
2020
;
13
(
3
):
464
473
24
Durkin
MS
,
Maenner
MJ
,
Baio
J
, et al
.
Autism spectrum disorder among US children (2002-2010): socioeconomic, racial, and ethnic disparities
.
Am J Public Health
.
2017
;
107
(
11
):
1818
1826
25
Durkin
MS
,
Maenner
MJ
,
Meaney
FJ
, et al
.
Socioeconomic inequality in the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder: evidence from a U.S. cross-sectional study
.
PLoS One
.
2010
;
5
(
7
):
e11551
26
Charman
T
,
Pickles
A
,
Simonoff
E
,
Chandler
S
,
Loucas
T
,
Baird
G
.
IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders: data from the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP)
.
Psychol Med
.
2011
;
41
(
3
):
619
627
27
Howard
J
,
Copeland
JN
,
Gifford
EJ
, et al
.
Brief report: classifying rates of students with autism and intellectual disability in North Carolina: roles of race and economic disadvantage
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2021
;
51
(
1
):
307
314
28
Kim
ET
,
Franz
L
,
Fannin
DK
,
Howard
J
,
Maslow
G
.
Educational classifications of autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability among school-aged children in North Carolina: associations with race, rurality, and resource availability
.
Autism Res
.
2021
;
14
(
5
):
1046
1060
29
Polyak
A
,
Kubina
RM
,
Girirajan
S
.
Comorbidity of intellectual disability confounds ascertainment of autism: implications for genetic diagnosis
.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet
.
2015
;
168
(
7
):
600
608
30
Bhasin
TK
,
Schendel
D
.
Sociodemographic risk factors for autism in a US metropolitan area
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2007
;
37
(
4
):
667
677
31
Delobel-Ayoub
M
,
Ehlinger
V
,
Klapouszczak
D
, et al
.
Socioeconomic disparities and prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability
.
PLoS One
.
2015
;
10
(
11
):
e0141964
32
Dunn
K
,
Rydzewska
E
,
Fleming
M
,
Cooper
SA
.
Prevalence of mental health conditions, sensory impairments and physical disability in people with co-occurring intellectual disabilities and autism compared with other people: a cross-sectional total population study in Scotland
.
BMJ Open
.
2020
;
10
(
4
):
e035280
33
Xie
S
,
Heuvelman
H
,
Magnusson
C
, et al
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders with and without intellectual disability by gestational age at birth in the Stockholm Youth Cohort: a Register Linkage Study
.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol
.
2017
;
31
(
6
):
586
594
34
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD)
.
35
Rice
CE
,
Baio
J
,
Van Naarden Braun
K
,
Doernberg
N
,
Meaney
FJ
,
Kirby
RS
;
ADDM Network
.
A public health collaboration for the surveillance of autism spectrum disorders
.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol
.
2007
;
21
(
2
):
179
190
36
Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2002 Principal Investigators
;
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders--autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 14 sites, United States, 2002
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2007
;
56
(
1
):
12
28
37
Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network Surveillance Year 2008 Principal Investigators
;
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders--Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 14 sites, United States, 2008
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2012
;
61
(
3
):
1
19
38
Baio
J
,
Wiggins
L
,
Christensen
DL
, et al
.
Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2014
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2018
;
67
(
6
):
1
23
39
Christensen
DL
,
Baio
J
,
Van Naarden Braun
K
, et al;
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC)
.
Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 Years--Autism and Developmental Disabilities Monitoring Network, 11 Sites, United States, 2012
.
MMWR Surveill Summ
.
2016
;
65
(
3
):
1
23
40
Zahorodny
W
,
Shenouda
J
,
Howell
S
, %
Rosato
NS
,
Peng
B
,
Mehta
U
.
Increasing autism prevalence in metropolitan New Jersey
.
Autism
.
2014
;
18
(
2
):
117
126
41
Maenner
MJ
,
Graves
SJ
,
Peacock
G
, %
Honein
MA
,
Boyle
CA
,
Dietz
PM
.
Comparison of 2 case definitions for ascertaining the prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among 8-year-old children
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2021
;
190
(
10
):
2198
2207
42
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
.
Bridged-race population estimates 1990-2018 request
.
Available at: http://wonder.cdc.gov/bridged-race-v2018.html. Accessed Jul 24, 2021
43
Bishaw
A
,
Benson
C
,
Shirider
E
,
Glassman
B
.
Changes in poverty rates and poverty areas over time: 2005 to 2019
.
44
Posserud
MB
,
Skretting Solberg
B
,
Engeland
A
,
Haavik
J
,
Klungsøyr
K
.
Male to female ratios in autism spectrum disorders by age, intellectual disability and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
.
Acta Psychiatr Scand
.
2021
;
144
(
6
):
635
646
45
Barger
B
,
Benevides
T
,
Rizk
S
, et al
.
Race/ethnic inequities in conjoint monitoring and screening for U.S. children 3 and under
.
Disabil Health J
.
2022
;
15
(
1
):
101179
46
Mandell
DS
,
Ittenbach
RF
,
Levy
SE
,
Pinto-Martin
JA
.
Disparities in diagnoses received prior to a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2007
;
37
(
9
):
1795
1802
47
Yuan
J
,
Li
M
,
Lu
ZK
.
Racial/ethnic disparities in the prevalence and trends of autism spectrum disorder in US children and adolescents
.
JAMA Netw Open
.
2021
;
4
(
3
):
e210771
48
Fombonne
E
,
Zuckerman
KE
.
Clinical profiles of black and white children referred for autism diagnosis
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2021
;
52
(
3
):
1120
1130
49
Jarquin
VG
,
Wiggins
LD
,
Schieve
LA
,
Van Naarden-Braun
K
.
Racial disparities in community identification of autism spectrum disorders over time; Metropolitan Atlanta, Georgia, 2000-2006
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2011
;
32
(
3
):
179
187
50
Gordillo
ML
,
Chu
A
,
Long
K
.
Mothers’ adjustment to autism: exploring the roles of autism knowledge and culture
.
J Pediatr Psychol
.
2020
;
45
(
8
):
877
886
51
Kang-Yi
CD
,
Grinker
RR
,
Mandell
DS
.
Korean culture and autism spectrum disorders
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2013
;
43
(
3
):
503
520
52
Samadi
SA
.
Parental beliefs and feelings about autism spectrum disorder in Iran
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2020
;
17
(
3
):
828
53
Dawson
G
,
Rogers
S
,
Munson
J
, et al
.
Randomized, controlled trial of an intervention for toddlers with autism: the Early Start Denver Model
.
Pediatrics
.
2010
;
125
(
1
):
e17
e23
54
Kasari
C
,
Gulsrud
AC
,
Wong
C
,
Kwon
S
,
Locke
J
.
Randomized controlled caregiver mediated joint engagement intervention for toddlers with autism
.
J Autism Dev Disord
.
2010
;
40
(
9
):
1045
1056
55
Nahmias
AS
,
Pellecchia
M
,
Stahmer
AC
,
Mandell
DS
.
Effectiveness of community- based early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder: a meta-analysis
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2019
;
60
(
11
):
1200
1209
56
Tsang
LPM
,
How
CH
,
Yeleswarapu
SP
,
Wong
CM
.
Autism spectrum disorder: early identification and management in primary care
.
Singapore Med J
.
2019
;
60
(
7
):
324
328
57
Anderson
DK
,
Liang
JW
,
Lord
C
.
Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2014
;
55
(
5
):
485
494

Supplementary data