BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES

Previously, in 30 Bangladeshi villages, 2 groups of children with iron-deficiency anemia (IDA) and nonanemic (NA) iron sufficiency aged 6 to 24 months participated in 2 parallel cluster randomized controlled trials of the effect of psychosocial stimulation on neurodevelopment. The intervention was composed of weekly play sessions at home for 9 months. All children with anemia received iron treatment of 6 months. The intervention improved the mental development of NA but not IDA groups. Six years after end line when the children were aged 8 to 9 years, we aimed to determine if benefits were sustained in the NA group or late-onset benefits emerged in the IDA group.

METHODS

We relocated 372 (90%) of the initial 412 children from all the clusters (villages), and assessed their IQ with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence-II, motor development, and school achievement including math, spelling, and reading. Analyses were by intention-to-treat, adjusting for clustering.

RESULTS

There was a significant interaction between anemia groups (IDA/NA) and intervention on IQ. The intervention benefitted the NA group’s Full-Scale IQ (effect size, 0.43 [95% confidence interval, 0.08–0.79]) and Perceptual Reasoning Index (effect size, 0.48 [95% confidence interval, 0.08–0.89]) but did not affect the IDA group’s outcomes. No other outcomes were significant.

CONCLUSIONS

The benefits from early childhood psychosocial stimulation on the NA group’s IQ, 6 years after intervention ended, adds to the limited evidence on the sustainability of benefits in low- and middle-income countries. Reasons for lack of effect in children with anemia are unknown.

Iron is important for myelination, the production of neurotransmitters, and carrying oxygen in hemoglobin.1  Iron deficiency is the most common cause of anemia24  and the most prevalent hematologic disorder in childhood.5,6  Iron-deficiency anemia (IDA) has been negatively associated with young children’s cognition, behavior, motor development, school achievement, and career choice in later life.714  However, the independent role of IDA in cognitive development in young children remains controversial because IDA is associated with many disadvantages likely to affect children’s development,12  and there are few randomized trials of iron supplementation in children with consistent benefits to cognition.11 

An estimated 249.4 million children aged <5 years are at risk for not reaching their developmental potential in low-and-middle-income countries (LMICs).15  Inadequate stimulation in the home, stunting, and iron deficiency were identified as key risk factors for child development.16,17  Many studies and reviews have shown that interventions with psychosocial stimulation alone or combined with nutritional supplementation benefit children’s development in the short term.1825  In particular, 6 Bangladeshi trials with disadvantaged children have shown improvement to their mental development.2631  However, the sustainability of benefits is uncertain. A recent review of LMIC studies found only a few followed children after the intervention finished and most were short term; only 2 studies followed participants aged >6 years (school entry).32  One in Jamaica found comprehensive benefits up to age 31 years33 ; the other in South Africa failed to find benefits at age 13 years.34  The review concluded that the results were inconclusive and more longitudinal data were needed.32  We located only 1 study that measured the impact of psychosocial stimulation on development of children with IDA. In Chile, 1 year of weekly home visiting with psychosocial stimulation for children aged 6 to 24 months with IDA improved their cognitive and social–emotional scores, whereas children without anemia showed no benefit.35  The finding of children without anemia showing no benefit was unusual because most similar interventions have shown short-term benefits.2631  The children selected were otherwise healthy and had normal birth weights and lengths; it may be that the level of stimulation in the home was already adequate.

In a previous Bangladeshi study, children with and without anemia aged 6 to 24 months participated in 2 parallel cluster randomized trials of psychosocial stimulation for 9 months. The children without anemia had better socioeconomic backgrounds than the children with anemia, and these were controlled in the analyses to examine the effects of anemia. Stimulation benefited mental development in nonanemic (NA) but not IDA groups, although all children with anemia received iron supplementation and were no longer anemic after intervention.29  It was hypothesized that children with anemia may take longer to respond than children without anemia, and improvements might appear later.

We therefore followed up the participants 6 years after the above trial ended. The aims were to determine if developmental benefits were sustained in the NA group and if any late-onset benefits emerged in the IDA group. The primary outcomes were children’s IQs and subscales; secondary outcomes were children’s school achievement, motor development, anthropometry hemoglobin, and the home environment. We also examined differences between children with and without anemia at enrollment.

The initial study was a cluster randomized controlled trial of psychosocial stimulation provided to children with IDA (serum transferrin receptor ≥5.0 mg/L and hemoglobin >80 and <110 g/L) and children who are NA and iron sufficient aged 6 to 24 months conducted in 30 villages. Eligible children were screened for anemia and iron status, and an average of 7 children with IDA were selected from each village. Children with obvious disabilities (8 children) and twins (2 pairs) were excluded from the evaluation. Children with severe anemia were referred for treatment. Within each village, every child with anemia was matched with a child without anemia for age (±6 months) and sex. Where more children were available, they were selected randomly. The villages were then randomized to stimulation or control. The resulting sample was 117 treated and 108 control children with anemia and 107 treated and 102 control children without anemia (Fig 1).

FIGURE 1

Medium-term follow-up.

FIGURE 1

Medium-term follow-up.

Close modal

At enrollment, the mean (SD) age of the children was 15 (5) months. There were 219 (50.5%) girls. The NA group had higher mean (SD) height-for-age z score −1.75 (1.25) than the IDA group −2.15 (1.10). The NA group’s mothers were better educated and had a higher BMI and family care indicator scores than those in the IDA group.

All children with or without anemia in the treatment villages received weekly play sessions at home for 9 months, using a curriculum designed in Jamaica36  and adapted for Bangladesh.26  All children with anemia in both stimulation and no stimulation groups received 30 mg of iron syrup daily for 6 months delivered to their home weekly. The children were assessed on the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, Second Edition, before and after the intervention, and blood samples were taken for iron status analysis. All children in the IDA group, except 1, improved in hemoglobin concentration (at least 10 g/L). Psychosocial stimulation improved the Mental Development Index (MDI) of the NA group but not the IDA group, and the interaction between anemic group and stimulation approached significance. The children with anemia had lower motor development scores and were less comfortable meeting the tester than the children without anemia at end line. There were no other anemia group differences, once imbalances in socioeconomic backgrounds were controlled. Further study details are available.29 

The children were relocated by field organizers, recruited from the community. They invited mother–child dyads to a nearby place to assess the children’s performance and interview the mothers or caregivers.

Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) was assessed with the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI)-II.37  The WASI was developed and standardized in the United States, and was adapted for this population to be culturally appropriate, but it was not standardized for Bangladesh. Therefore, the scores should not be compared with children elsewhere and were only used to assess group differences. The adapted WASI-II has been used previously in Bangladesh.38  The vocabulary and similarity subscales were used to measure the children’s Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), and the block design and matrix reasoning subscales were used to measure the Performance Reasoning Index (PRI). The VCI and PRI together formed the FSIQ.

Motor development was assessed with the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (age-band 2 for 7–10 years).39  This test has also been used in Bangladesh previously.38 

School achievement was measured using a locally developed tool based on the Wide Range Achievement Test.40  The test included reading, spelling, and mathematics, and was administered individually. The test was previously used in Bangladesh with primary school children.38,41 

The quality of the home environment was assessed using the Middle Childhood inventory of Home Observation for Measurement of Environment.42  The questionnaire was translated and adapted for use in Bangladeshi children.

Children’s anthropometry, height, weight, mid-upper arm circumference, and occipitofrontal circumference were measured by the assessors using World Health Organization standard procedure.43 

Hemoglobin status of the children was assessed from venous blood using cyanmethemoglobin.

Sociodemographic information was collected by the assessors through interviewing mothers or caregivers. The information included the number of household members; age of mother; family structure; parental education and occupation; the quality of roof, floor, and walls of the house; and availability of electricity in the residence.

Five assessors (4 females, 1 male) having at least a bachelor’s degree assessed the child development outcomes and interviewed the caregivers for demographic details and the home environment. The assessors were recruited from outside the study area and conducted assessments rotating through intervention and control clusters, and were blind to the children’s group assignment. A psychologist with 10 years’ experience in child development assessment trained the assessors for 1 month on the assessment tools and questionnaires. The assessors began testing when they achieved >90% agreement with the trainer in each test.

The protocol was approved by the Research and Ethical Review Committees of the institutional review board of icddr,b, and written consent was obtained from the parents of the children.

To examine differences of the children’s background characteristics by groups, we used 2-way analysis of variance. We conducted univariate analysis to determine the differences among the 4 groups (NA stimulation [NA-stim] versus NA no stimulation [NA-no stim], IDA stimulation [IDA-stim] versus IDA no stimulation [IDA-no stim]). Similarly, univariate analysis was also conducted to assess differences between IDA versus NA groups on outcome variables at follow-up. We also used t test to check if the lost and tested children at follow-up were different on any of the measured variables.

The enrollment MDI and follow-up IQ and its subscales were internally standardized using the SD of the population. Intention-to-treat analyses were used to detect all treatment effects. We performed multiple linear regression analysis to assess the effects of stimulation on all primary outcomes. Covariates for the models were selected if the background characteristics were different by treatment groups on enrollment. Inverse probability-weighted estimation was used to correct for possible bias because of loss to follow-up. These weights were used in all multiple regression models of the impact of intervention. We included enrollment MDI, the anemia group (NA/IDA), an interaction term between stimulation and anemia groups in the regression model, and adjusted for testers’ effect and cluster design. We also conducted multiple regression analysis for IDA and NA groups separately. Statistical significance was set at P < .05. All data were analyzed using Stata (version 13.0).

Out of 412 targeted children who completed the original study, 372 (90%) were assessed at this follow-up. Reasons for loss to follow-up were migration (n = 34) and deaths (n = 5) because of drowning, dog bite, and diarrhea. One child could not be tested because of a behavioral problem and was dropped. This resulted in a final sample of 102 (92.7%) children in the IDA-stim group, 94 (88.7%) in the IDA-no stim group, 92 (88.5%) in the NA-stim group, and 84 (91.3%) in the NA-no stim group (Fig 1).

There were no significant differences in the background characteristics of the tested and lost children at enrollment (Supplemental Table 5).

The mean (SD) ages of the children of NA and IDA groups were 106.5 (7.34) and 106.0 (6.6) months, respectively. There were more girls in the NA group compared with the IDA group. The mothers of the NA group had higher education and home stimulation scores than the mothers of the IDA group. The NA group still had higher hemoglobin than the IDA group, but their height-for-age z scores were no longer different. (Table 1).

TABLE 1

Socioeconomic Characteristics, Anthropometry and Hemoglobin Level of the 4 Groups at Medium-Term Follow-Up

NA Group, Mean ± SD or n (%)IDA Group, Mean ± SD or n (%)Pa
Socioeconomic CharacteristicsNo Stimulation, n = 84Stimulation, n = 92All NA, n = 176No Stimulation, n = 94Stimulation, n = 102All IDA, n = 196NA Versus IDAStimulation Versus no StimulationAnemiaa Stimulation
 Age, mo 105.3 ± 5.3 107.6 ± 8.7 106.5 ± 7.3 104.6 ± 4.5 107.8 ± 7.8 106.3 ± 6.6 .73 <.01 .59 
 Sex, female 46 (54.8) 53 (57.6) 99 (56.2) 42 (44.7) 50 (49.0) 92 (46.9) <.01 .56 .72 
 Mothers’ education, y 6.4 ± 3.1 5.5 ± 3.5 6.14 ± 3.63 4.5 ± 3.6 5.0 ± 3.1 5.3 ± 3.9 <.01 .63 .06 
 Fathers’ education, y 4.7 ± 4.2 5.2 ± 3.75 5.23 ± 4.1 4.6 ± 3.7 4.2 ± 3.7 4.7 ± 4.1 .17 .94 .30 
 Housing index 7.2 ± 1.4 7.5 ± 1.2 7.33 ± 1.3 7.3 ± 1.2 7.4 ± 1.4 7.3 ± 1.3 .94 .31 .17 
 Crowding index 2.8 ± 1.4 3.0 ± 1.5 2.87 ± 1.4 2.7 ± 1.6 3.2 ± 1.5 2.9 ± 1.6 .67 .04 .35 
 HOME 29.1 ± 6.4 28.8 ± 6.8 28.98 ± 6.5 27.3 ± 6.3 27.0 ± 5.5 27.1 ± 5.9 <.01 .67 .93 
Anthropometry          
Mother’s BMI, weight (kg)/height (m2) (n = 352) 21.8 ± 4.1 22.9 ± 3.7 22.36 ± 3.9 21.8 ± 3.8 22.3 ± 3.8 22.0 ± 3.8 .46 .38 .05 
 Children’s WAZ (n = 356) −1.8 ± 1.1 −1.7 ± 1.1 −1.75 ± 1.1 −1.8 ± 1.0 −1.9 ± 1.2 −1.85 ± 1.07 .41 .86 .21 
 Children’s HAZ (n = 371) −1.5 ± 0.9 −1.3 ± 1.0 −1.41 ± 0.9 −1.4 ± 0.1 −1.4 ± 1.0 −1.4 ± 0.9 .75 .45 .40 
 Children’s BAZ (n = 371) −1.4 ± 1.2 −1.3 ± 1.3 −1.39 ± 1.2 −1.4 ± 1.2 −1.5 ± 1.2 −1.45 ± 1.2 .33 .86 .50 
Hemoglobin level, mg/dL 12.9 ± 0.7 12.8 ± 0.7 12.8 ± 0.7 12.5 ± 0.9 12.6 ± 0.7 12.6 ± 0.8 <.01 .75 .56 
NA Group, Mean ± SD or n (%)IDA Group, Mean ± SD or n (%)Pa
Socioeconomic CharacteristicsNo Stimulation, n = 84Stimulation, n = 92All NA, n = 176No Stimulation, n = 94Stimulation, n = 102All IDA, n = 196NA Versus IDAStimulation Versus no StimulationAnemiaa Stimulation
 Age, mo 105.3 ± 5.3 107.6 ± 8.7 106.5 ± 7.3 104.6 ± 4.5 107.8 ± 7.8 106.3 ± 6.6 .73 <.01 .59 
 Sex, female 46 (54.8) 53 (57.6) 99 (56.2) 42 (44.7) 50 (49.0) 92 (46.9) <.01 .56 .72 
 Mothers’ education, y 6.4 ± 3.1 5.5 ± 3.5 6.14 ± 3.63 4.5 ± 3.6 5.0 ± 3.1 5.3 ± 3.9 <.01 .63 .06 
 Fathers’ education, y 4.7 ± 4.2 5.2 ± 3.75 5.23 ± 4.1 4.6 ± 3.7 4.2 ± 3.7 4.7 ± 4.1 .17 .94 .30 
 Housing index 7.2 ± 1.4 7.5 ± 1.2 7.33 ± 1.3 7.3 ± 1.2 7.4 ± 1.4 7.3 ± 1.3 .94 .31 .17 
 Crowding index 2.8 ± 1.4 3.0 ± 1.5 2.87 ± 1.4 2.7 ± 1.6 3.2 ± 1.5 2.9 ± 1.6 .67 .04 .35 
 HOME 29.1 ± 6.4 28.8 ± 6.8 28.98 ± 6.5 27.3 ± 6.3 27.0 ± 5.5 27.1 ± 5.9 <.01 .67 .93 
Anthropometry          
Mother’s BMI, weight (kg)/height (m2) (n = 352) 21.8 ± 4.1 22.9 ± 3.7 22.36 ± 3.9 21.8 ± 3.8 22.3 ± 3.8 22.0 ± 3.8 .46 .38 .05 
 Children’s WAZ (n = 356) −1.8 ± 1.1 −1.7 ± 1.1 −1.75 ± 1.1 −1.8 ± 1.0 −1.9 ± 1.2 −1.85 ± 1.07 .41 .86 .21 
 Children’s HAZ (n = 371) −1.5 ± 0.9 −1.3 ± 1.0 −1.41 ± 0.9 −1.4 ± 0.1 −1.4 ± 1.0 −1.4 ± 0.9 .75 .45 .40 
 Children’s BAZ (n = 371) −1.4 ± 1.2 −1.3 ± 1.3 −1.39 ± 1.2 −1.4 ± 1.2 −1.5 ± 1.2 −1.45 ± 1.2 .33 .86 .50 
Hemoglobin level, mg/dL 12.9 ± 0.7 12.8 ± 0.7 12.8 ± 0.7 12.5 ± 0.9 12.6 ± 0.7 12.6 ± 0.8 <.01 .75 .56 

BAZ, BMI z score; HOME, Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment; HAZ, height for age z score; WAZ, weight for age z score.

a

Analysis of variance.

Table 2 presents the mean scores of primary outcomes by 4 groups using univariate analysis. There were small differences between the NA and IDA groups in all IQ scales and school achievement outcomes except in motor development (data not shown).

TABLE 2

Children’s Test Scores at Medium-Term Follow-Up in the 4 Groups

Outcome VariablesNA GroupIDA Group
Stimulation, n = 92No Stimulation, n = 84Stimulation, n = 102No Stimulation, n = 94
Mean ± SDPaMean ± SDPa
WASI-II       
 FSIQ 65.7 ± 7.0 62.8 ± 6.5 .03 63.1 ± 7.2 62.2 ± 6.1 .99 
 PRI 74.4 ± 8.2 71.0 ± 7.6 .02 71.8 ± 7.6 70.2 ± 7.4 .90 
 VCI 61.3 ± 10.5 58.5 ± 8.2 .31 57.4 ± 10.3 58.2 ± 8.3 .99 
School achievement       
 Word reading 96.5 ± 22.6 92.5 ± 21.9 .99 88.1 ± 24.5 91.5 ± 23.0 .99 
 Word spelling 89.8 ± 21.4 88.2 ± 20.8 .99 80.8 ± 22.9 83.0 ± 21.3 .99 
 Math computation 80.6 ± 14.6 78.0 ± 16.5 .99 74.0 ± 14.8 77.0 ± 17.2 .99 
Motor development       
 MABC (n = 345) 25.2 ± 6.9 22.0 ± 7.1 .02 24.0 ± 6.8 22.6 ± 7.6 .99 
Outcome VariablesNA GroupIDA Group
Stimulation, n = 92No Stimulation, n = 84Stimulation, n = 102No Stimulation, n = 94
Mean ± SDPaMean ± SDPa
WASI-II       
 FSIQ 65.7 ± 7.0 62.8 ± 6.5 .03 63.1 ± 7.2 62.2 ± 6.1 .99 
 PRI 74.4 ± 8.2 71.0 ± 7.6 .02 71.8 ± 7.6 70.2 ± 7.4 .90 
 VCI 61.3 ± 10.5 58.5 ± 8.2 .31 57.4 ± 10.3 58.2 ± 8.3 .99 
School achievement       
 Word reading 96.5 ± 22.6 92.5 ± 21.9 .99 88.1 ± 24.5 91.5 ± 23.0 .99 
 Word spelling 89.8 ± 21.4 88.2 ± 20.8 .99 80.8 ± 22.9 83.0 ± 21.3 .99 
 Math computation 80.6 ± 14.6 78.0 ± 16.5 .99 74.0 ± 14.8 77.0 ± 17.2 .99 
Motor development       
 MABC (n = 345) 25.2 ± 6.9 22.0 ± 7.1 .02 24.0 ± 6.8 22.6 ± 7.6 .99 

MABC, Movement Assessment Battery for Children.

a

Analysis of variance.

The NA-stim group had higher FSIQ (P = .03) and PRI (P < .02) scores than the NA-no stim group, but there were no significant differences between IDA-stim and IDA-no stim groups. There were no differences in the mean scores of IQ, school achievement, and motor development in the anemia groups.

On assessing the treatment effects on IQ with multiple regression analysis with all 4 groups included, there was a significant stimulation effect on FSIQ (B, 0.29; 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.04 to 0.54) and PRI (B, 0.32 CI 0.07 to 0.57), but no difference between the anemia groups. However, there was a significant interaction between the stimulation and anemia groups in FSIQ and VCI (Table 3).

TABLE 3

Multiple Regression Analysis of IQ on Treatment (Stimulation) and Anemia Group (IDA/NA) in All Children Adjusting for Covariates (n = 372)

FSIQPRIVCI
B (95% CI)PB (95% CI)PB (95% CI)P
Child’s age at follow-up, y 0.003 (−0.02 to 0.02) .76 −0.01 (−0.03 to 0.01) .22 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.04) .23 
Enrollment MDI, z scores 0.22 (0.10 to 0.34) <.01 0.16 (0.06 to 0.25) <.01 0.21 (0.07 to 0.34) <.01 
Mothers’ education at enrollment, y 0.05 (0.02 to 0.09) <.01 0.03 (0.001 to 0.06) .04 0.05 (0.02 to 0.09) <.01 
Height for age z score at enrollment 0.10 (0.01 to 0.19) .03 0.08 (0.01 to 0.16) .03 0.06 (−0.04 to 0.16) .24 
FCI at enrollment 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.04) .32 0.01 (−0.02 to 0.04) .45 0.01 (−0.02 to 0.05) .37 
Housing index at follow-up 0.07 (−0.01 to 0.16) .09 0.02 (0.06 to 0.11) .60 0.09 (−0.01 to 0.18) .08 
NA/IDA group 0.15 (−0.02 to 0.24) .12 0.13 (−0.04 to 0.29) .13 0.12 (−0.03 to 0.26) .13 
Stimulation/control 0.29 (0.04 to 0.54) .02 0.32 (0.07 to 0.57) .02 0.12 (−0.17 to 0.41) .40 
Anemia stimulation interaction 0.33 (0.02 to 0.63) .04 −0.01 (−0.03 to 0.01) .23 0.43 (0.14 to 0.73) <.01 
Adjusted R2 0.23 0.15 0.20 
FSIQPRIVCI
B (95% CI)PB (95% CI)PB (95% CI)P
Child’s age at follow-up, y 0.003 (−0.02 to 0.02) .76 −0.01 (−0.03 to 0.01) .22 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.04) .23 
Enrollment MDI, z scores 0.22 (0.10 to 0.34) <.01 0.16 (0.06 to 0.25) <.01 0.21 (0.07 to 0.34) <.01 
Mothers’ education at enrollment, y 0.05 (0.02 to 0.09) <.01 0.03 (0.001 to 0.06) .04 0.05 (0.02 to 0.09) <.01 
Height for age z score at enrollment 0.10 (0.01 to 0.19) .03 0.08 (0.01 to 0.16) .03 0.06 (−0.04 to 0.16) .24 
FCI at enrollment 0.01 (−0.01 to 0.04) .32 0.01 (−0.02 to 0.04) .45 0.01 (−0.02 to 0.05) .37 
Housing index at follow-up 0.07 (−0.01 to 0.16) .09 0.02 (0.06 to 0.11) .60 0.09 (−0.01 to 0.18) .08 
NA/IDA group 0.15 (−0.02 to 0.24) .12 0.13 (−0.04 to 0.29) .13 0.12 (−0.03 to 0.26) .13 
Stimulation/control 0.29 (0.04 to 0.54) .02 0.32 (0.07 to 0.57) .02 0.12 (−0.17 to 0.41) .40 
Anemia stimulation interaction 0.33 (0.02 to 0.63) .04 −0.01 (−0.03 to 0.01) .23 0.43 (0.14 to 0.73) <.01 
Adjusted R2 0.23 0.15 0.20 

Model: Age at follow-up, enrollment MDI (z scores), mothers’ education, child height for age z score at enrollment, testers, Family Care Indicator (FCI) at enrollment, and clusters. Outcome scores were internally standardized (z scores) using SD of population. CI, confidence interval. FCI, family care indicator.

We therefore analyzed the anemia groups separately. In the NA group, there were significant stimulation effects on FSIQ (B, 0.43; CI, 0.08 to 0.79) and PRI (B, 0.48; CI, 0.08 to 0.89), but not on VCI. In the IDA group, there were no significant treatment effects (Table 4).

TABLE 4

Treatment Effects on the Primary Outcomes in NA and IDA Groups Separately

FSIQPRIVCI
B (95% CI)PB (95% CI)PB (95% CI)P
NA group (stimulation effect) 0.43 (0.08 to 0.79) .02 0.48 (0.08 to 0.89) .02 0.27 (−0.06 to 0.61) .11 
Anemic group (stimulation effect) 0.14 (−0.12 to 0.39) .29 0.15 (0.11 to 0.41) .25 −0.06 (−0.40 to 0.28) .73 
FSIQPRIVCI
B (95% CI)PB (95% CI)PB (95% CI)P
NA group (stimulation effect) 0.43 (0.08 to 0.79) .02 0.48 (0.08 to 0.89) .02 0.27 (−0.06 to 0.61) .11 
Anemic group (stimulation effect) 0.14 (−0.12 to 0.39) .29 0.15 (0.11 to 0.41) .25 −0.06 (−0.40 to 0.28) .73 

Model: Age at follow-up, enrollment MDI (z scores), mothers’ education, child height for age z score at enrollment, Family Care Indicator (FCI) at enrollment, testers, and clusters were adjusted in analysis. Outcome scores were internally standardized (z scores) using SD of the population. CI, confidence interval. FCI, family care indicator.

There were no significant treatment effects in either the NA or IDA group in reading, spelling, mathematics, or Movement Assessment Battery for Children (Supplemental Table 6).

Children who were either with or without anemia in early childhood and participated in a cluster randomized trial of psychosocial stimulation intervention29  were followed up 6 years after intervention ended, at age 8 to 9 years. Benefits had been found to the NA group’s mental development immediately after intervention and benefits to the FSIQ and the PRI were present at follow-up but there was no effect on motor development or school achievement. It is encouraging that benefits from early intervention to IQ were sustained and the findings add to the scant literature on medium term sustainability of benefits in LMICs. Acquiring literacy and mathematics skills depends to some extent on the quality of teaching, which may not have been of a high standard in these children’s schools.

Conversely, in children with anemia, no benefits were found on development immediately after intervention and none on IQ, motor scores, or school achievement at follow-up. Therefore, the only difference at follow-up between the NA and IDA groups in any of the developmental outcomes was the lack of response to intervention in children with anemia compared with benefits found in children without anemia. Many studies of psychosocial stimulation in early childhood have shown child development benefits immediately after intervention; however, the medium and long-term benefits have rarely been examined, especially in LMICs.32 

Follow-up of the Jamaican study, which showed comprehensive adult benefits, found no school achievement benefits at age 11 years,44  but benefits were apparent at 17 years.45  It is therefore possible that improvements in school achievement in the current study may appear at a later age in the NA group.

A few short-term follow-up studies up to 6 years of age in LMICs have found sustained child development benefits: in Pakistan at age 4 years,46  in Uganda at age 3 years47  and in Jamaica at age 6 years;48  whereas, in Colombia at age 4 years, no benefits were found.49  There is considerable debate as to whether IDA detrimentally affects young children’s development, although there is a well-established association.9  As expected, initially, the IDA group had several disadvantages compared with the NA group, including IDA children being more likely to be stunted and have poorer social backgrounds.29  However, children with anemia had similar scores to the children without anemia on all development outcomes at enrollment once background differences were controlled. At the end of the initial trial, the children’s MDI was similar in both groups, but the IDA group had lower scores on the Psychomotor Development Index compared with the NA group. Most interestingly, they did not benefit from the intervention, unlike the NA group; the significant interaction between treatment and the anemia group at follow-up confirms this finding.

In the original study, children with anemia in both treatment and control groups received iron syrup for 6 months because of ethical concerns. Therefore, it is not possible to know the developmental response to iron. Nevertheless, another study conducted in Bangladeshi children found that iron supplementation alone had a beneficial effect on orientation–engagement and both iron and zinc supplementation promoted motor development.50  A systematic review and meta-analysis documented that iron supplementation on children with anemia aged 6 to 24 months improved hemoglobin status, but the developmental benefits were uncertain.11  There are very few studies that followed long-term effects of iron supplementation on cognitive development.

A limitation in the current study is that it cannot help to clarify the controversy over the effects of iron supplementation on child development,11  because the control group was treated with iron because of ethical concerns. However, it was remarkable that there was no difference in developmental outcomes between the anemia groups at follow-up except in their response to stimulation. The findings suggest that children with anemia may be less responsive to improvements in the environment. However, the IDA group was more disadvantaged than the NA group and it is possible that the other unmeasured disadvantages, and not IDA per se, affected their response to intervention.

Another limitation is that, whereas both the groups with anemia received iron treatment, the NA groups did not get a placebo. It is possible that giving iron reduced the visit time spent on child development and may have reduced the intervention’s effectiveness. In addition, the IDA control group also received weekly visits when we delivered the iron syrups, whereas the NA control group did not receive visits. These visits may have increased the attention to the IDA-no stim group. It therefore remains uncertain why the IDA group failed to benefit from stimulation.

The strength of the study was that it comprised 2 parallel cluster randomized trials and both IDA and NA groups had the same home visitors and the same intervention. Furthermore, the background characteristics of the stimulated and control groups were balanced and the attrition was small.

Children who were not anemic and participated in a home visiting psychosocial stimulation intervention in early childhood had sustained benefits on their IQ and perceptual reasoning at 8 to 9 years of age, 6 years after the intervention. The persistence of benefits at 8 to 9 years provide further support for the promotion of child development interventions in rural children. Further follow-up to assess longer-term sustainability would be helpful. In contrast, matched children with IDA showed no benefit. This lack of response should be explored further.

We thank all the families who participated in the study. We also thank the assessors who measured the children’s development and the local field organizers who located the participants. We thank the commitment of Swedish International Development Assistance for its research efforts. We also thank the governments of Bangladesh, Canada, Sweden, and the United Kingdom for providing core/unrestricted support.

Mr Hossain conceptualized and designed the study, analyzed the data, coordinated and supervised data collection, and wrote the first draft of the manuscript; Dr Tofail conceptualized and designed the study, and coordinated and supervised data collection; Dr Mehrin coordinated and supervised data collection; Dr Hamadani conceptualized and designed the study, analyzed the data, and coordinated and supervised data collection; and all authors critically reviewed, revised, and approved the final manuscript as submitted, and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

This study is registered at ClinicalTrials.gov, #NCT02801721, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/NCT02801721. Deidentified data will not be made available. All requests to obtain the original data must be addressed to Research Administration of the host organization. The Department of Research Administration maintains a data repository. A copy of the complete data set will remain in the repository. Interested researchers may contact corresponding author for full data access.

FUNDING: Funded by Swedish International Development Assistance, grant #54100089.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST DISCLOSURES: The authors have indicated they have no conflicts of interest relevant to this article to disclose.

FSIQ

Full-Scale IQ

IDA

iron-deficiency anemia

IDA-no stim

iron-deficiency anemia no stimulation

IDA-stim

iron-deficiency anemia stimulation

LMIC

low- and middle-income country

MDI

Mental Development Index

NA

nonanemic

NA-no stim

nonanemic no stimulation

NA-stim

nonanemic stimulation

PRI

Performance Reasoning Index

VCI

Verbal Comprehensive Index

WASI

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence

1
Chmielewska
A
,
Chmielewski
G
,
Domellöf
M
,
Lewandowski
Z
,
Szajewska
H
.
Effect of iron supplementation on psychomotor development of nonanemic, exclusively or predominantly breastfed infants: a randomized, controlled trial
.
BMJ Open
.
2015
;
5
(
11
):
e009441
2
De Benoist
B
,
Cogswell
M
,
Egli
I
,
McLean
E
.
Worldwide Prevalence of Anemia 1993–2005; WHO Global Database Of Anemia
.
Geneva
:
WHO
;
2008
3
Pasricha
SR
,
Hasan
MI
,
Braat
S
, et al
.
Benefits and risks of iron interventions in infants in rural Bangladesh
.
N Engl J Med
.
2021
;
385
(
11
):
982
995
4
World Health Organization
.
Serum Ferritin Concentrations for the Assessment of Iron Status and Iron Deficiency in Populations
.
Geneva
:
WHO
;
2011
5
Lopez
A
,
Cacoub
P
,
Macdougall
IC
,
Peyrin-Biroulet
L
.
Iron deficiency anemia
.
Lancet
.
2016
;
387
(
10021
):
907
916
6
Dallman
PR
,
Yip
R
,
Johnson
C
.
Prevalence and causes of anemia in the United States, 1976 to 1980
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
1984
;
39
(
3
):
437
445
7
Lozoff
B
,
Jimenez
E
,
Hagen
J
,
Mollen
E
,
Wolf
AW
.
Poorer behavioral and developmental outcome more than 10 years after treatment for iron deficiency in infancy
.
Pediatrics
.
2000
;
105
(
4
):
E51
8
Lozoff
B
,
Beard
J
,
Connor
J
,
Barbara
F
,
Georgieff
M
,
Schallert
T
.
Long-lasting neural and behavioral effects of iron deficiency in infancy
.
Nutr Rev
.
2006
;
64
(
5 Pt 2
):
S34
S43
,
discussion S72–S91
9
Grantham-McGregor
S
,
Ani
C
.
A review of studies on the effect of iron deficiency on cognitive development in children
.
J Nutr
.
2001
;
131
(
2S-2
):
649S
668S
,
discussion 666S-668S
10
Larson
LM
,
Phiri
KS
,
Pasricha
S-R
.
Iron and cognitive development: what is the evidence?
Ann Nutr Metab
.
2017
;
71
(
Suppl 3
):
25
38
11
Pasricha
SR
,
Hayes
E
,
Kalumba
K
,
Biggs
BA
.
Effect of daily iron supplementation on health in children aged 4–23 months: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials
.
Lancet Glob Health
.
2013
;
1
(
2
):
e77
e86
12
Lu
C
,
Black
MM
,
Richter
LM
.
Risk of poor development in young children in low-income and middle-income countries: an estimation and analysis at the global, regional, and country level
.
Lancet Glob Health
.
2016
;
4
(
12
):
e916
e922
13
Lind
T
,
Lönnerdal
B
,
Stenlund
H
, et al
.
A community-based, randomized controlled trial of iron and zinc supplementation in Indonesian infants: effects on growth and development
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2004
;
80
(
3
):
729
736
14
Yalçin
SS
,
Yurdakök
K
,
Açikgöz
D
,
Özmert
E
.
Short-term developmental outcome of iron prophylaxis in infants
.
Pediatr Int
.
2000
;
42
(
6
):
625
630
15
Black
MM
,
Walker
SP
,
Fernald
LCH
, et al.
Lancet Early Childhood Development Series Steering Committee
.
Early childhood development coming of age: science through the life course
.
Lancet
.
2017
;
389
(
10064
):
77
90
16
Walker
SP
,
Wachs
TD
,
Gardner
JM
, et al.
International Child Development Steering Group
.
Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries
.
Lancet
.
2007
;
369
(
9556
):
145
157
17
Walker
SP
,
Wachs
TD
,
Grantham-McGregor
S
, et al
.
Inequality in early childhood: risk and protective factors for early child development
.
Lancet
.
2011
;
378
(
9799
):
1325
1338
18
Pitchik
HO
,
Tofail
F
,
Rahman
M
, et al
.
A holistic approach to promoting early child development: a cluster randomized trial of a group-based, multicomponent intervention in rural Bangladesh
.
BMJ Glob Health
.
2021
;
6
(
3
):
e004307
19
Grantham-McGregor
SM
,
Powell
CA
,
Walker
SP
,
Himes
JH
.
Nutritional supplementation, psychosocial stimulation, and mental development of stunted children: the Jamaican Study
.
Lancet
.
1991
;
338
(
8758
):
1
5
20
Gardner
JM
,
Powell
CA
,
Baker-Henningham
H
,
Walker
SP
,
Cole
TJ
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
.
Zinc supplementation and psychosocial stimulation: effects on the development of undernourished Jamaican children
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2005
;
82
(
2
):
399
405
21
Waber
DP
,
Vuori-Christiansen
L
,
Ortiz
N
, et al
.
Nutritional supplementation, maternal education, and cognitive development of infants at risk of malnutrition
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
1981
;
34
(
Suppl 4
):
807
813
22
Vazir
S
,
Engle
P
,
Balakrishna
N
, et al
.
Cluster-randomized trial on complementary and responsive feeding education to caregivers found improved dietary intake, growth and development among rural Indian toddlers
.
Matern Child Nutr
.
2013
;
9
(
1
):
99
117
23
Andrew
A
,
Attanasio
O
,
Augsburg
B
, et al
.
Effects of a scalable home‐visiting intervention on child development in slums of urban India: evidence from a randomized controlled trial
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2020
;
61
(
6
):
644
652
24
Hartinger
SM
,
Lanata
CF
,
Hattendorf
J
, et al
.
Impact of a child stimulation intervention on early child development in rural Peru: a cluster randomized trial using a reciprocal control design
.
J Epidemiol Community Health
.
2017
;
71
(
3
):
217
224
25
Attanasio
OP
,
Fernández
C
,
Fitzsimons
EO
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
,
Meghir
C
,
Rubio-Codina
M
.
Using the infrastructure of a conditional cash transfer program to deliver a scalable integrated early child development program in Colombia: cluster randomized controlled trial
.
BMJ
.
2014
;
349
:
g5785
26
Hamadani
JD
,
Huda
SN
,
Khatun
F
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
.
Psychosocial stimulation improves the development of undernourished children in rural Bangladesh
.
J Nutr
.
2006
;
136
(
10
):
2645
2652
27
Nahar
B
,
Hossain
MI
,
Hamadani
JD
, et al
.
Effects of a community-based approach of food and psychosocial stimulation on growth and development of severely malnourished children in Bangladesh: a randomized trial
.
Eur J Clin Nutr
.
2012
;
66
(
6
):
701
709
28
Hamadani
JD
,
Mehrin
SF
,
Tofail
F
, et al
.
Integrating an early childhood development programme into Bangladeshi primary health-care services: an open-label, cluster-randomized controlled trial
.
Lancet Glob Health
.
2019
;
7
(
3
):
e366
e375
29
Tofail
F
,
Hamadani
JD
,
Mehrin
F
,
Ridout
DA
,
Huda
SN
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
.
Psychosocial stimulation benefits development in nonanemic children but not in anemic, iron-deficient children
.
J Nutr
.
2013
;
143
(
6
):
885
893
30
Mehrin
SF
,
Hasan
MI
,
Tofail
F
, et al
.
Integrating a group-based, early childhood parenting intervention into primary health care services in rural Bangladesh: a cluster-randomized controlled trial
.
Front Pediatr
.
2022
;
10
:
886542
31
Hossain
SJ
,
Roy
BR
,
Sujon
HM
, et al
.
Effects of integrated psychosocial stimulation (PS) and unconditional cash transfer (UCT) on children’s development in rural Bangladesh: a cluster randomized controlled trial
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2022
;
293
:
114657
32
Jeong
J
,
Pitchik
HO
,
Fink
G
.
Short-term, medium-term and long-term effects of early parenting interventions in low-and middle-income countries: a systematic review
.
BMJ Glob Health
.
2021
;
6
(
3
):
e004067
33
Walker
SP
,
Chang
SM
,
Wright
AS
,
Pinto
R
,
Heckman
JJ
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
.
Cognitive, psychosocial, and behaviour gains at age 31 years from the Jamaica early childhood stimulation trial
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2022
;
63
(
6
):
626
635
34
St Clair
MC
,
Skeen
S
,
Marlow
M
,
Tomlinson
M
.
Relationships between concurrent language ability and mental health outcomes in a South African sample of 13-year-olds
.
PLoS One
.
2019
;
14
(
9
):
e0221242
35
Lozoff
B
,
Smith
JB
,
Clark
KM
,
Perales
CG
,
Rivera
F
,
Castillo
M
.
Home intervention improves cognitive and social-emotional scores in iron-deficient anemic infants
.
Pediatrics
.
2010
;
126
(
4
):
e884
e894
36
Grantham-McGregor
S
,
Smith
JA
.
Extending the Jamaican early childhood development intervention
.
J Appl Res Child
.
2016
;
7
(
2
):
4
37
Stano
JF
.
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
.
Rehabil Couns Bull
.
2004
;
48
(
1
):
56
57
38
Ali
H
,
Hamadani
J
,
Mehra
S
, et al
.
Effect of maternal antenatal and newborn supplementation with vitamin A on cognitive development of school-aged children in rural Bangladesh: a follow-up of a placebo-controlled, randomized trial
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2017
;
106
(
1
):
77
87
39
Henderson
SE
,
Sugden
D
,
Barnett
AL
.
Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (Movement ABC-2)
.
London, UK
:
The Phsychological Corporation
;
2007
40
Wilkinson
GS
,
Robertson
GJ
.
Wide Range Achievement Test 4
.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol
.
1993
41
Huda
SN
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
,
Rahman
KM
,
Tomkins
A
.
Biochemical hypothyroidism secondary to iodine deficiency is associated with poor school achievement and cognition in Bangladeshi children
.
J Nutr
.
1999
;
129
(
5
):
980
987
42
Bradley
RH
,
Caldwell
BM
,
Rock
SL
,
Hamrick
HM
,
Harris
P
.
Home observation for measurement of the environment: development of a home inventory for use with families having children 6 to 10 years old
.
Contemp Educ Psychol
.
1988
;
13
(
1
):
58
71
43
Hamadani
JD
,
Tofail
F
,
Nermell
B
, et al
.
Critical windows of exposure for arsenic-associated impairment of cognitive function in pre-school girls and boys: a population-based cohort study
.
Int J Epidemiol
.
2011
;
40
(
6
):
1593
1604
44
Chang
SM
,
Walker
SP
,
Grantham-McGregor
S
,
Powell
CA
.
Early childhood stunting and later behavior and school achievement
.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry
.
2002
;
43
(
6
):
775
783
45
Walker
SP
,
Chang
SM
,
Powell
CA
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
.
Effects of early childhood psychosocial stimulation and nutritional supplementation on cognition and education in growth-stunted Jamaican children: prospective cohort study
.
Lancet
.
2005
;
366
(
9499
):
1804
1807
46
Yousafzai
AK
,
Obradović
J
,
Rasheed
MA
, et al
.
Effects of responsive stimulation and nutrition interventions on children’s development and growth at age 4 years in a disadvantaged population in Pakistan: a longitudinal follow-up of a cluster-randomized factorial effectiveness trial
.
Lancet Glob Health
.
2016
;
4
(
8
):
e548
e558
47
Atukunda
P
,
Muhoozi
GKM
,
van den Broek
TJ
, et al
.
Child development, growth and microbiota: follow-up of a randomized education trial in Uganda
.
J Glob Health
.
2019
;
9
(
1
):
010431
48
Walker
SP
,
Chang
SM
,
Younger
N
,
Grantham-McGregor
SM
.
The effect of psychosocial stimulation on cognition and behavior at 6 years in a cohort of term, low-birth weight Jamaican children
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2010
;
52
(
7
):
e148
e154
49
Andrew
A
,
Attanasio
O
,
Fitzsimons
E
,
Grantham-McGregor
S
,
Meghir
C
,
Rubio-Codina
M
.
Impacts 2 years after a scalable early childhood development intervention to increase psychosocial stimulation in the home: a follow-up of a cluster randomized controlled trial in Colombia
.
PLoS Med
.
2018
;
15
(
4
):
e1002556
50
Black
MM
,
Baqui
AH
,
Zaman
K
, et al
.
Iron and zinc supplementation promote motor development and exploratory behavior among Bangladeshi infants
.
Am J Clin Nutr
.
2004
;
80
(
4
):
903
910

Supplementary data