CONTEXT

Very preterm birth (<32 weeks) is associated with increased risk of developmental disorders. Emerging evidence suggests children born 32 to 38 weeks might also be at risk.

OBJECTIVES

To determine the relative risk and prevalence of being diagnosed with, or screening positive for, developmental disorders in children born moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term compared with term (≥37 weeks) or full term (39–40/41 weeks).

DATA SOURCES

Medline, Embase, Psychinfo, Cumulative Index of Nursing, and Allied Health Literature.

STUDY SELECTION

Reported ≥1 developmental disorder, provided estimates for children born 32 to 38 weeks.

DATA EXTRACTION

A single reviewer extracted data; a 20% sample was second checked. Data were pooled using random-effects meta-analyses.

RESULTS

Seventy six studies were included. Compared with term born children, there was increased risk of most developmental disorders, particularly in the moderately preterm group, but also in late preterm and early term groups: the relative risk of cerebral palsy was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 14.1 (95% confidence intervals [CI]: 12.3–16.0), 34 to 36 weeks: 3.52 (95% CI: 3.16–3.92) and 37 to 38 weeks: 1.44 (95% CI: 1.32–1.58).

LIMITATIONS

Studies assessed children at different ages using varied criteria. The majority were from economically developed countries. All were published in English. Data were variably sparse; subgroup comparisons were sometimes based on single studies.

CONCLUSIONS

Children born moderately preterm are at increased risk of being diagnosed with or screening positive for developmental disorders compared with term born children. This association is also demonstrated in late preterm and early term groups but effect sizes are smaller.

Preterm birth (<37 weeks) is associated with increased risk of developmental disorders, defined as: “heterogeneous conditions that share a disturbance in the acquisition of basic developmental skills in a chronologically appropriate manner.”1 

Most literature focuses on outcomes of children born very (<32 weeks), or extremely preterm (<28 weeks).2,3  However these represent only 15% of preterm births globally.4  Emerging evidence suggests early term birth (37–38 weeks) may also affect development.5,6 

The aim of this review was to determine the relative risk and prevalence of developmental disorders in children born between 32 and 38 weeks’ gestation, compared with term born children. Although previous reviews have explored some aspects of this question,710  to our knowledge there are no meta-analyses that have explored outcomes for moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term children and considered all developmental disorders.

Understanding the epidemiology of developmental disorders among children born between 32 and 38 weeks is important, not least because birth at this stage is common: in the United States in 2020, 27.8% of births were early term, 7.4% late preterm (34–36 weeks), and 1.2% moderately preterm (32–33 weeks) (compared with 0.6% extremely preterm).11  Knowing which disorders are most prevalent among these children could improve targeting of resources and antenatal counseling and potentially avoid delayed diagnosis and missed opportunities for intervention.1215 

This systematic review was registered with PROSPERO (CRD42021298773) and reported according to PRISMA guidelines.16  The population of interest was children born between 32 and 38 weeks, aged 2 to 17 years at assessment, born after 1996, when antenatal steroids administration in preterm labor became routine practice.17  Comparator groups were (where possible) children born at full term (39–40/41 weeks), or if this data were not presented, at term (≥37 weeks) . The outcomes were developmental disorders as per the National Institute for Health and Care Excellence,18  which in some cases were subdivided further to allow meaningful comparison: cerebral palsy (CP); developmental coordination disorder (DCD); visual impairment; hearing impairment; sleep apnea; oro-motor feeding problems; social, emotional, and behavioral problems subdivided into: global social, emotional, and behavioral problems, internalizing behaviors, externalizing behaviors, and social problems; attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); autism spectrum disorder (ASD); developmental delay subdivided into: global developmental delay, language delay, motor developmental delay, and cognitive developmental delay; cognitive impairment; executive function problems; low educational achievement, subdivided into: not “school ready” aged ≤5 years, low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years, and low educational achievement aged 12 to 17 years; and special educational needs (including physical disabilities which affect learning as well as learning impairments).19 

Searches were conducted in Medline, Embase, Psychinfo, Cumulative Index of Nursing and Allied Health Literature on February 10, 2022 (updated November 22, 2022). The search terms are listed in Supplemental Table 4. In addition, a Google scholar search was conducted on February 14, 2022. Initially the advanced search feature of Google Scholar was used, but the resulting studies were not relevant. Therefore, a basic search was performed using the question: “What is the risk of neurodevelopmental disorder in children born between 32 and 38 weeks gestation?”. The top 50 results were exported. Reference lists of included studies were searched for additional studies that met the inclusion criteria, as follows: at least 1 developmental disorder identified on a validated questionnaire, standardized test, or physician diagnosis; provided estimates for children born moderately or late preterm, or early term.

One author (K.P.) reviewed all titles, abstracts, and full texts using Covidence software.20  Twenty percent were independently reviewed by a second author (C.C.). There was substantial agreement; 93.6% and Cohen’s κ 0.62 at abstract screening; 98.4% and Cohen’s κ 0.85 at full text review.21  Disagreements were resolved by discussion.

Where key data were missing, authors were contacted by e-mail; if no response was received, the study was excluded. Where studies used the same cohort, the most recent publication was included, unless an older study provided the results in a binary format, enabling prevalence calculation (Supplemental Table 5 for summary). The full list of exclusion criteria can be found in the study protocol (CRD42021298773).

Data were extracted by K.P. and a 20% sample was cross-checked by C.C. for accuracy. See Supplemental Table 3 for data extraction form. Where only odds ratios were presented, the study authors were contacted to request the raw data.

The Newcastle-Ottawa scale was used to assess quality with scores categorized as poor (0–2), fair (3–5), or good (6–9) (Supplemental Fig 11).22 

An unadjusted pooled relative risk of each outcome by gestational age was calculated using random effects meta-analysis because of expected heterogeneity, quantified using the I2 statistic. The number of children with each disorder, the number without, and the total number of participants (split into gestational subgroups) was used in the calculations. If raw numbers were not presented (or supplied on request), the paper’s unadjusted calculated effect sizes (relative risk) were used. When there were 0 cases, a continuity correction of 0.5 was applied. For continuous measures, studies were pooled using Hedge’s G standardized mean difference. Signs were changed where necessary.23  Prevalence was pooled by gestational age with a fixed effect model using the inverse-variance method. The Freeman Tukey double arcsine transformation was specified where necessary to stabilize variances.24,25  Publication bias was assessed using Egger’s test and funnel plots.

Analyses were undertaken by gestational age groupings: moderately preterm (32–33 weeks); late preterm (34–36 weeks); early term (37–38 weeks)5,26,27 ; moderate-to-late preterm (32–36 weeks), but only if results were presented in this format without further breakdown.

Where results were not presented as above, broader groupings were used: 32 weeks was coded as 32–33 weeks; 35–36 weeks was coded as 34–36 weeks; 33–36 weeks, 32–34 weeks, 32–35 weeks, or 32–37 weeks were all coded as 32–36 weeks; and 32–34 weeks and 35–36 weeks were combined into 32–36 weeks.

Where data were available for children born at 32–33 weeks and 34–36 weeks, the 32–36 week category was not presented.

Sensitivity analysis was undertaken for relative risk of CP, global developmental delay, and educational outcomes aged 6 to 11 years by comparing results when studies using a ≥37 week term comparison group (as opposed to a 39–40/41 weeks comparison group) were included and excluded.

The initial search identified 11 630 records with 17 further studies from reference screening and updated searches. As shown in Fig 1, 9028 studies were excluded at the first stage and 1462 studies were full text screened; 1386 were excluded, resulting in 76 studies included. There was full agreement between the 2 researchers conducting data extraction.

FIGURE 1

Study selection process.

FIGURE 1

Study selection process.

Close modal

Key characteristics of the included studies are summarized in Table 1. Several studies reported multiple outcomes. All were cohort or cross sectional studies. Sample sizes ranged from 83 to 1 390 601 and covered the full age range (2–17 years). The majority were from economically developed countries. There were 59 studies rated as “good” quality and 15 as “fair’” (Supplemental Table 7). Two studies were cross sectional, therefore 2 of the Newcastle Ottawa Scale questions did not apply and so were not classified as good, fair, or poor quality. Twenty four meta-analyses were conducted, summarized in Table 2. No papers reported oro-motor feeding problems.

TABLE 1

Key Characteristics of Included Studies Organized by Country, Then by Year of Publication

Record IDParticipants’ Country of OriginTotal Participants Included in Meta-analysisAge at AssessmentBirth Year of Participants% Male (whole cohort unless 1% difference in gestational age groups)Ethnicity of Participants Using Same Terminology as the Studies (whole cohort or term group)Developmental Disorders ReportedStudy Quality
Baron 200953  United States 34–36 wk: 60; ≥37 wk: 35 3 y 2004–2005 34–36 wk 45%; ≥37 wk 60% NR Cognition Fair 
Morse 200954  United States 34–36 wk: 7152; ≥37 wk: 152 661 (SEN) 4 y 1996–1997 34–36 wk: 52.2%; ≥37 wk: 51.3% Black 21.32%; white 55.69%; other 22.98% SEN, education - not school ready Good 
Petrini 200955  United States 34–36 wk: 8341; 37–41weeks: 128 955 (CP) 1–5 y 2000–2004 34–36 wk: 54.4%; 37–41 wk: 50.9% Hispanic 24.5%; Asian 18.2%; white 41.5%; unknown 8.5%; Black 7.3% CP, DD Good 
Baron 201056  United States 35–36 wk: 118; ≥37 wk: 100 3 y 2004–2006 35–36 wk: 50%; ≥37 wk: 60% Caucasian 72% multiracial 14%; Hispanic: 6%, Asian 4%; African/African American 3%; other 1% Cognition Good 
Woythaler 201157  United States 34–36 wk: 1200; ≥37 wk: 6300 2 y 2001 34–36 wk: 52.6%; ≥37 wk: 51.4% White 81.4%; Black 14.3%; other 4.3% DD Good 
Baron 201258  United States 34–36 wk: 52; ≥37 wk: 195 3 y 2004 and 2006 34–36 wk: 51%; ≥37 wk: 59% Caucasian 72.7% Cognition Fair 
Curry 201259  United States 32–33 wk: 3973; 34–36 wk: 21 835; 37–38 wk: 79 228; ≥39 wk: 229 626 Up to 3 y 1999–2001 51.3% (NR by gestation) Hispanic 34.3%, Asian/Pacific Islander 11.9%, Black, non-Hispanic 27.6%, white, non-Hispanic 25.9%, other 0.2% DD Good 
Talge 201260  United States 3–5 y: 34–36 wk: 96; ≥37 wk: 388; 6–9 y: 34–36 wk: 56; ≥37 wk: 222 3–5 y and 6–9 y 1998–2004 48% White or other 76%; African American 24% SEB, ADHD Good 
Baron 201461  United States 34–36 wk: 410; ≥37 wk: 192 3 y 2004–2008 34–36 wk: 55.4%; ≥37 wk: 53.1% White 67.7%; other 24%; Hispanic 6.3%; African American: 2.1% Cognition Good 
Brumbaugh 201462  United States 32–34 wk: 39; 38–42 wk: 44; (cognition) 4 y 2005–2006 32–34 wk:59%; 38–42 wk: 50% NR Executive function, cognition Fair 
Kuzniewicz 201463  United States 34–36 wk: 11 945; ≥37 wk: 177 129 Minimum 2 y 2000–2007 NR White: 42%; Hispanic: 24%; Asian: 21%; Black 7%; other 6% ASD Good 
Richards 201564  United States 34–36 wk: 25 850; 37–41 wk: 29 4076 6–8 y 1998–2002 34–36 wk: 52.9%; 37–41 wk: 50.4% White non-Hispanic 55.6%; Black non-Hispanic 34.4%; Hispanic 10.0% Education–aged 6–11 y Good 
Woythaler 201565  United States 34–36 wk: 950; ≥37 wk: 4900 5 y 2001 34–36 wk: 53.6%, ≥37 wk: 50.4% White 81.1%; Black: 14.6%; other: 4.3% Education- not school ready Good 
Hodel 201666  United States 32–36 wk: 45; 37–42 wk: 44; (cognition) 4.5 y Not stated 32–36 wk: 51.1%; 37–42 wk: 54.3% Caucasian 92% Cognition, executive function Fair 
Crockett 202267  United States 34–36 wk: 1339; 39–41 wk: 15 203 Up to age 8 2000–2005 34–36 wk: 53.6%; 39–41 wk: 51.1% NR Education- not school ready and aged 6–11 y, ADHD Good 
Lingasubramanian 202268  United States 37–38 wk: 400; 39–41 wk: 1022 9 y 1998–2000 52% Non-Hispanic white 22%; Non-Hispanic Black 48%; Hispanic 27%; another non-white 4% ADHD Good 
Wehby 202269  United States 32–36 wk: 75 017; ≥37 wk: 888 623 6–16 y 2002–2010 51% White 94%; Black 3%; other3% Education Good 
Robaei 200670  Australia 32–36 wk: 115; ≥37 wk: 1343 (vision) 6 y 1997–1998 32–36 wk: 52%; ≥37 wk: 51% White 65.9%; Southeast Asian 15.5%; other 18.5% Vision Fair 
Raynes-Greenow 201271  Australia 32–36 wk: 22 039; ≥37 wk: 377 952 2–6 y 2000–2004 51.4% NR Sleep apnea Good 
Schneider 201472  Australia 33–36 wk: 63; 37–41 wk: 44 12 y (mean) 1996–1997 33–36 wk: 57%; 37–41 wk: 52% NR Cognition Good 
Smithers 201573  Australia 37–38 wk: 3374; 39–40 wk: 7505 5 y (median) 2004 Whole cohort 50%; (NR by gestation) Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander 3.3% Education- not school ready Good 
Cheong 201728  Australia 32–36 wk: 176; ≥37 wk: 150; (SEB - social) 2 y 2009–2012 32–36 wk: 47.8%; ≥37 wk: 53.2% NR DD, SEB, CP Good 
Searle 201774  Australia 32–36 wk: 838; 37–38 wk: 3762; 40 wk: 6224 8 y 2000–2002 50.2% (NR by gestation) NR Education–aged 6–11 y Good 
Hanly 201846  Australia 34–36 wk: 3932; 37–38 wk: 20 951; 39–40 wk: 43 199 5 y 2004–2007 Whole cohort 50.7% (NR by gestation) Aboriginal 7% Education- not school ready Good 
Brown 201975  Australia 34–36 wk: 76; 37–38 wk: 295; 39–40 wk: 471 8–9 y 2005–2006 52.0% (NR by gestation) NR Education– aged 6–11years Good 
Dhamrait 202176  Australia 32–36 wk: 3709; ≥37 wk: 60 675 4–5 y 2004 32–36 wk 52.8%; 37–38 wk 50.8%; 39–40 wk 49.4% Caucasian 82%; other 12.3%; Indigenous Australian 5.7% Education- not school ready Good 
Poulsen 201377  UK 32–33 wk: 123; 34–36 wk: 646; 37–38 wk: 2188; 39–41 wk: 8096 (cognition) 3 y 2000–2002 32–33 wk: 59.7%; 39–41 wk: 50.6% White British: 80% to 89% Education- not school ready, cognition Good 
Guy 201578  UK 32–33 wk: 82; 34–36 wk: 539; ≥ 37 wk: 749 2 y 2009–2010 NR 98% White in the group with a negative ASD screen ASD Fair 
Johnson 2015a3  UK 32–33 wk: 85; 34–36 wk: 545; 37–42 wk: 750 2 y 2009–2010 32–36 wk: 53.8%; term: 50.2% White 82.5%, Asian or Asian British 11.2%, Black or Black British 4.4%, Chinese or other 1.0%, Mixed 1.0% DD Good 
Johnson 2015b79  UK 32–33 wk: 84; 34–36 wk: 541; ≥37 wk: 760 2 y 2009–2010 32–36 wk: 53.8%; term: 50.2% White 78.6%; Asian or Asian British 4.7%; Black or Black British 3.6% Mixed 2.0%; Chinese or other 1.0%; unknown 0.2% SEB Good 
Cronin 201680  UK 32–33 wk: 164; 34–36 wk: 691; 37–38 wk: 2864; 39–41 wk: 9811 5 y 2002 32–33 wk: 57.3%; 34–36 wk: 53.7%; 37–38 wk: 51.9%; 37–41 wk: 50.6% White British 87.9% SEB Good 
Pettinger 202081  UK 32–33 wk: 65; 34–36 wk: 478; 37–38 wk: 2355; 39–41 wk: 7269 4 y 2007–2010 52% English as additional language: 45% Education - not school ready Good 
Alterman 202182  UK 32–33 wk: 135; 34–36 wk: 732; 37–38 wk: 2460; 39–40 wk 6051 11 y 2000–2002 Whole cohort: 51%; 34–36 wk: 64% White 87.1%, other 12.9% SEN Good 
Alterman 202283  UK 32–33 wk: 76; 34–36 wk: 401; 37–38 wk: 1408; 39–41 wk: 4896 (education aged 6–11 y) 11 y 2000–2001 32–33 wk: 63.6%; 34–36 wk: 51.4%; 37–38 wk: 52.5%; 39–41 wk: 49.8% White: 85.2% Education aged 6–11 y, education aged 12–17 y Good 
Libuy 202284  UK 32–33 wk: 2227; 34–36 wk: 13 385; 37–38 wk: 57 955; 39–40 wk: 156 376 11 y 2004–2005 32–33 wk: 55.1%; 34–36 wk: 54.6%; 37–38 wk: 52.2%; 39–40 wk: 50.5% White: 78.6%, Asian 10.1%; Black 5.0%, any other ethnic group 1.4%, mixed 4.9% SEN, education aged 6–11 y Good 
Van Baar 200985  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 377; ≥37 wk: 182 8 y 1996–1999 32–36 wk: 52%; ≥37 wk: 47% Dutch 91% Cognition, SEB Good 
Bul 201286  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 348; ≥37 wk: 182 8 y 1996–1999 32–36 wk: 51.1%, ≥37 wk: 47.8% NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Cserjesi 201287  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 248; 38–41 wk: 130 6.9 y (mean) 2002–2003 32–36 wk: 55.6%; 38–41 wk: 44.6% NR Cognition Good 
de Jong 201588  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 116; 37–41 wk: 98; (cognition) 4 y 2010–2011 32–36 wk 57.8%; >37 wk: 45.5% Dutch 95.9% Cognition, DD, SEB Fair 
Potijk 201589  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 915; ≥37 wk: 543 3 y 2002–2003 54.2% Dutch: 94.6% SEB Good 
Hornman 201790  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 644; 38–41 wk: 375 4 y 2002–2003 34–36 wk: 58.3%; 38–41 wk: 47.1% Non-Dutch background 4.3% DD Good 
Hua 202191  China 32–33 wk: 2322; 34–36 wk: 12 915; 37–38 wk: 38 875; 39–40 wk: 76 501 3–5 y 2013–2016 32–33 wk: 54.0%; 34–36 wk: 55.3%; 37–38 wk: 55.3%; 39–40 wk: 51.6% Han Chinese 99% Motor Good 
You 2019a92  China 34–36 wk: 102; 37–42 wk: 153 24–30 mo 2011–2013 34–36 wk: 66.7%; 37–42 wk: 61.4% NR DD, CP Good 
You 2019b93  China 34–36 wk: 112; 37–42 wk: 179 2 y 2013–2015 34–36 wk: 54.5%; 37–42 wk: 47.5% NR DD, CP Fair 
Zhou 202045  China 32–37 wk: 83; 37–42 wk:1665 1 mo–5 y 2011–2016 32–37 wk: 51.8%; 37–42 wk: 53.8% NR DD Good 
Stene-Larsen 201494  Norway 34–36 wk: 1673; 37–38 wk: 7109; 39–41 wk: 30 641 3 y 1999–2008 34–36 wk: 51.3%; 37–38 wk: 48.6%; 39–41 wk: 50.4% NR DD Good 
Strand 201395  Norway 32–36 wk: 23 763; 37–40 wk: 522 551 4 y (minimum) 1996–2006 NR - only reported for pre-eclampsia versus no pre-eclampsia NR CP Good 
Ask 201896  Norway 34–36 wk: 1755; 37–38 wk: 6732; 40 wk: 11 753 (SEB) 5 y 1999–2008 Whole cohort 51.3% (NR by gestation) NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Zambrana 202197  Norway Total: 26 769 3 y 1999–2008 51.5% (NR by gestation) NR DD Good 
Demestre 201698  Spain 34–36 wk: 90; 38–41 wk: 89 4 y 2009 34–36 wk: 61%; 38–41 wk: 55% NR DD Good 
Oros 201499  Spain 34–36 wk: 6; ≥37 wk: 96 6–13 y 1997–2005 34–36 wk: 40.9%; term 52.6% Caucasian 99.3% Cognition Fair 
Pérez-Pereira 2014100  Spain 34–36 wk: 47; ≥37 wk: 36 2.5 y Unclear 34–36 wk: 56.5%; ≥37 wk: 51.4% NR Speech Fair 
Pérez-Pereira 2020101  Spain 32–33 wk: 31; 34–36 wk: 42; ≥37 wk: 33 5 y Unclear 52% NR Cognition Good 
Klassen 2004102  Canada 33–37 wk: 341; >37 wk: 259 (vision) 3.5 y 1996–1997 33–37 wk: 45.2%; >37 wk: 40.2% NR DD, hearing, vision Fair 
Leavey 2013103  Canada 32–33 wk: 2373; 34–36 wk: 13 108; 37–38 wk: 51 307; 39–41 wk: 146 467 Minimum 3 y 1998–2004 49% NR ASD Good 
Faleschini 2020104  Canada 32–36 wk: 89; ≥37 wk: 1841 4 y 1997–1998 32–36 wk: 52%; ≥37 wk: 50% Born in Canada 87% SEB, ADHD Good 
Chen 2020105  Sweden 37–38 wk: 132 997; 39–41 wk: 530 988 3 y 1998–2009 51.1% NR Cognition Good 
Beer 2022106  Sweden 32–36 wk: 47 859; 37–41 wk: 1 071 729 3–15 y 2002–2014 NR NR ADHD Good 
Chen 2022107  Sweden 32–36 wk: 77 986; 37–38 wk: 346 859; 39–40 wk: 965 756 0–16 y (median 9.4 y) 1998–2016 50.4% (NR by gestation) NR CP Good 
Larsen 2021108  Denmark 32–33 wk: 6067; 34–36 wk: 29 821; 37–41 wk: 531 996 (SEN) 4–6 y 1997–2013 NR - it is only reported for each disorder not each gestation NR SEN, CP Good 
Zhu 2012109  Denmark 32–33 wk: 123; 34–36 wk: 721; 37–38 wk: 3519; 39–40 wk: 11 743 7 y 1996–2002 51% NR Motor Good 
Larroque 2008110  France 32 wk: 484; 39–40 wk: 389 (vision) 5 y 1997 52% male NR Cognition, hearing, vision, CP Good 
Bailhache 2022111  France 32–36 wk: 421; ≥37 wk: 4164 9 y 2011 50.9% NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Voigt 2012112  Germany 32–37 wk: 88; ≥38 wk: 86 2 y 2007–2008 32–37 wk 51%; ≥38 wk 47% NR Cognition, SEB, ADHD Fair 
Reuner 2015113  Germany 33–36 wk 54; ≥37 wk 38 2 y 2008–2009 33–36 wk: 50%; ≥37 wk: 47% NR Cognition Fair 
Darlow 2009114  New Zealand 33–36 wk: 112; ≥37 wk: 101 2 y 2001–2002 33–36 wk: 61%; term admissions 59% Māori 5.9% CP, DD Good 
Berry 2018115  New Zealand 33–36 wk: 19 089; 37–38 wk: 70 026; 39–40 wk:180 987 (vision) 6–11 y 1998–2000 and 2005–2015 51.4% European 67%; Pacific Islander 12%; Māori 21% SEN, education aged 12–17 y, hearing, vision Good 
Yang 2010116  Belarus 38 wk: 2100; 39–41 wk: 11 074 6 y 1996–1997 38 wk: 53.8%; 39 wk: 50.8%; 40 wk: 51.7% NR Cognition Good 
Polic 2017117  Croatia 34–36 wk: 126; 37–40 wk: 131 6–12 y 2002–2008 34–36 wk: 56.3%; 37–40 wk: 59.5% NR SEB Good 
Hirvonen 2015118  Finland 32–33 wk: 4862; 34–36 wk: 28 152; ≥37 wk: 671 988 7 y 1996–2008 32–33 wk: 54.9%; 34–36 wk: 54.2%; ≥37 wk: 50.8% NR CP Good 
Drougia 2007119  Greece 34–36 wk 594; ≥37 wk: 1719 Minimum 2 y 1997–2003 NR NR CP Good 
Tso 2022120  Hong Kong 33–36 wk: 22 811; 37–41 wk: 330 087 6–17 y, mean 11 y 7 mo 2004–2014 52% NR ADHD Good 
Patil 2014121  India 32–36 wk: 100; ≥37 wk: 100 4–6 y unclear Preterm 59%; term 61% NR DD Fair 
Marotta 2020122  Italy 34–36 wk: 53; ≥37 wk: 53 5–11 y (mean 8) 2002–2010 39.6% NR SEB Fair 
Higa Diez 2016123  Japan 34–36 wk: 645; 37–38 wk: 5103; 39–41 wk: 11752 8 y 2001 34–36 wk 60.1%; 37–38 wk 55.3%; 39–41 wk 49.7% NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Al-Haddad 2017124  Lebanon 32–36 wk: 51; ≥ 37 wk: 69 4.9 y (mean) 2006–2014 55% NR Vision Fair 
Record IDParticipants’ Country of OriginTotal Participants Included in Meta-analysisAge at AssessmentBirth Year of Participants% Male (whole cohort unless 1% difference in gestational age groups)Ethnicity of Participants Using Same Terminology as the Studies (whole cohort or term group)Developmental Disorders ReportedStudy Quality
Baron 200953  United States 34–36 wk: 60; ≥37 wk: 35 3 y 2004–2005 34–36 wk 45%; ≥37 wk 60% NR Cognition Fair 
Morse 200954  United States 34–36 wk: 7152; ≥37 wk: 152 661 (SEN) 4 y 1996–1997 34–36 wk: 52.2%; ≥37 wk: 51.3% Black 21.32%; white 55.69%; other 22.98% SEN, education - not school ready Good 
Petrini 200955  United States 34–36 wk: 8341; 37–41weeks: 128 955 (CP) 1–5 y 2000–2004 34–36 wk: 54.4%; 37–41 wk: 50.9% Hispanic 24.5%; Asian 18.2%; white 41.5%; unknown 8.5%; Black 7.3% CP, DD Good 
Baron 201056  United States 35–36 wk: 118; ≥37 wk: 100 3 y 2004–2006 35–36 wk: 50%; ≥37 wk: 60% Caucasian 72% multiracial 14%; Hispanic: 6%, Asian 4%; African/African American 3%; other 1% Cognition Good 
Woythaler 201157  United States 34–36 wk: 1200; ≥37 wk: 6300 2 y 2001 34–36 wk: 52.6%; ≥37 wk: 51.4% White 81.4%; Black 14.3%; other 4.3% DD Good 
Baron 201258  United States 34–36 wk: 52; ≥37 wk: 195 3 y 2004 and 2006 34–36 wk: 51%; ≥37 wk: 59% Caucasian 72.7% Cognition Fair 
Curry 201259  United States 32–33 wk: 3973; 34–36 wk: 21 835; 37–38 wk: 79 228; ≥39 wk: 229 626 Up to 3 y 1999–2001 51.3% (NR by gestation) Hispanic 34.3%, Asian/Pacific Islander 11.9%, Black, non-Hispanic 27.6%, white, non-Hispanic 25.9%, other 0.2% DD Good 
Talge 201260  United States 3–5 y: 34–36 wk: 96; ≥37 wk: 388; 6–9 y: 34–36 wk: 56; ≥37 wk: 222 3–5 y and 6–9 y 1998–2004 48% White or other 76%; African American 24% SEB, ADHD Good 
Baron 201461  United States 34–36 wk: 410; ≥37 wk: 192 3 y 2004–2008 34–36 wk: 55.4%; ≥37 wk: 53.1% White 67.7%; other 24%; Hispanic 6.3%; African American: 2.1% Cognition Good 
Brumbaugh 201462  United States 32–34 wk: 39; 38–42 wk: 44; (cognition) 4 y 2005–2006 32–34 wk:59%; 38–42 wk: 50% NR Executive function, cognition Fair 
Kuzniewicz 201463  United States 34–36 wk: 11 945; ≥37 wk: 177 129 Minimum 2 y 2000–2007 NR White: 42%; Hispanic: 24%; Asian: 21%; Black 7%; other 6% ASD Good 
Richards 201564  United States 34–36 wk: 25 850; 37–41 wk: 29 4076 6–8 y 1998–2002 34–36 wk: 52.9%; 37–41 wk: 50.4% White non-Hispanic 55.6%; Black non-Hispanic 34.4%; Hispanic 10.0% Education–aged 6–11 y Good 
Woythaler 201565  United States 34–36 wk: 950; ≥37 wk: 4900 5 y 2001 34–36 wk: 53.6%, ≥37 wk: 50.4% White 81.1%; Black: 14.6%; other: 4.3% Education- not school ready Good 
Hodel 201666  United States 32–36 wk: 45; 37–42 wk: 44; (cognition) 4.5 y Not stated 32–36 wk: 51.1%; 37–42 wk: 54.3% Caucasian 92% Cognition, executive function Fair 
Crockett 202267  United States 34–36 wk: 1339; 39–41 wk: 15 203 Up to age 8 2000–2005 34–36 wk: 53.6%; 39–41 wk: 51.1% NR Education- not school ready and aged 6–11 y, ADHD Good 
Lingasubramanian 202268  United States 37–38 wk: 400; 39–41 wk: 1022 9 y 1998–2000 52% Non-Hispanic white 22%; Non-Hispanic Black 48%; Hispanic 27%; another non-white 4% ADHD Good 
Wehby 202269  United States 32–36 wk: 75 017; ≥37 wk: 888 623 6–16 y 2002–2010 51% White 94%; Black 3%; other3% Education Good 
Robaei 200670  Australia 32–36 wk: 115; ≥37 wk: 1343 (vision) 6 y 1997–1998 32–36 wk: 52%; ≥37 wk: 51% White 65.9%; Southeast Asian 15.5%; other 18.5% Vision Fair 
Raynes-Greenow 201271  Australia 32–36 wk: 22 039; ≥37 wk: 377 952 2–6 y 2000–2004 51.4% NR Sleep apnea Good 
Schneider 201472  Australia 33–36 wk: 63; 37–41 wk: 44 12 y (mean) 1996–1997 33–36 wk: 57%; 37–41 wk: 52% NR Cognition Good 
Smithers 201573  Australia 37–38 wk: 3374; 39–40 wk: 7505 5 y (median) 2004 Whole cohort 50%; (NR by gestation) Aboriginal and/or Torres Strait Islander 3.3% Education- not school ready Good 
Cheong 201728  Australia 32–36 wk: 176; ≥37 wk: 150; (SEB - social) 2 y 2009–2012 32–36 wk: 47.8%; ≥37 wk: 53.2% NR DD, SEB, CP Good 
Searle 201774  Australia 32–36 wk: 838; 37–38 wk: 3762; 40 wk: 6224 8 y 2000–2002 50.2% (NR by gestation) NR Education–aged 6–11 y Good 
Hanly 201846  Australia 34–36 wk: 3932; 37–38 wk: 20 951; 39–40 wk: 43 199 5 y 2004–2007 Whole cohort 50.7% (NR by gestation) Aboriginal 7% Education- not school ready Good 
Brown 201975  Australia 34–36 wk: 76; 37–38 wk: 295; 39–40 wk: 471 8–9 y 2005–2006 52.0% (NR by gestation) NR Education– aged 6–11years Good 
Dhamrait 202176  Australia 32–36 wk: 3709; ≥37 wk: 60 675 4–5 y 2004 32–36 wk 52.8%; 37–38 wk 50.8%; 39–40 wk 49.4% Caucasian 82%; other 12.3%; Indigenous Australian 5.7% Education- not school ready Good 
Poulsen 201377  UK 32–33 wk: 123; 34–36 wk: 646; 37–38 wk: 2188; 39–41 wk: 8096 (cognition) 3 y 2000–2002 32–33 wk: 59.7%; 39–41 wk: 50.6% White British: 80% to 89% Education- not school ready, cognition Good 
Guy 201578  UK 32–33 wk: 82; 34–36 wk: 539; ≥ 37 wk: 749 2 y 2009–2010 NR 98% White in the group with a negative ASD screen ASD Fair 
Johnson 2015a3  UK 32–33 wk: 85; 34–36 wk: 545; 37–42 wk: 750 2 y 2009–2010 32–36 wk: 53.8%; term: 50.2% White 82.5%, Asian or Asian British 11.2%, Black or Black British 4.4%, Chinese or other 1.0%, Mixed 1.0% DD Good 
Johnson 2015b79  UK 32–33 wk: 84; 34–36 wk: 541; ≥37 wk: 760 2 y 2009–2010 32–36 wk: 53.8%; term: 50.2% White 78.6%; Asian or Asian British 4.7%; Black or Black British 3.6% Mixed 2.0%; Chinese or other 1.0%; unknown 0.2% SEB Good 
Cronin 201680  UK 32–33 wk: 164; 34–36 wk: 691; 37–38 wk: 2864; 39–41 wk: 9811 5 y 2002 32–33 wk: 57.3%; 34–36 wk: 53.7%; 37–38 wk: 51.9%; 37–41 wk: 50.6% White British 87.9% SEB Good 
Pettinger 202081  UK 32–33 wk: 65; 34–36 wk: 478; 37–38 wk: 2355; 39–41 wk: 7269 4 y 2007–2010 52% English as additional language: 45% Education - not school ready Good 
Alterman 202182  UK 32–33 wk: 135; 34–36 wk: 732; 37–38 wk: 2460; 39–40 wk 6051 11 y 2000–2002 Whole cohort: 51%; 34–36 wk: 64% White 87.1%, other 12.9% SEN Good 
Alterman 202283  UK 32–33 wk: 76; 34–36 wk: 401; 37–38 wk: 1408; 39–41 wk: 4896 (education aged 6–11 y) 11 y 2000–2001 32–33 wk: 63.6%; 34–36 wk: 51.4%; 37–38 wk: 52.5%; 39–41 wk: 49.8% White: 85.2% Education aged 6–11 y, education aged 12–17 y Good 
Libuy 202284  UK 32–33 wk: 2227; 34–36 wk: 13 385; 37–38 wk: 57 955; 39–40 wk: 156 376 11 y 2004–2005 32–33 wk: 55.1%; 34–36 wk: 54.6%; 37–38 wk: 52.2%; 39–40 wk: 50.5% White: 78.6%, Asian 10.1%; Black 5.0%, any other ethnic group 1.4%, mixed 4.9% SEN, education aged 6–11 y Good 
Van Baar 200985  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 377; ≥37 wk: 182 8 y 1996–1999 32–36 wk: 52%; ≥37 wk: 47% Dutch 91% Cognition, SEB Good 
Bul 201286  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 348; ≥37 wk: 182 8 y 1996–1999 32–36 wk: 51.1%, ≥37 wk: 47.8% NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Cserjesi 201287  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 248; 38–41 wk: 130 6.9 y (mean) 2002–2003 32–36 wk: 55.6%; 38–41 wk: 44.6% NR Cognition Good 
de Jong 201588  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 116; 37–41 wk: 98; (cognition) 4 y 2010–2011 32–36 wk 57.8%; >37 wk: 45.5% Dutch 95.9% Cognition, DD, SEB Fair 
Potijk 201589  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 915; ≥37 wk: 543 3 y 2002–2003 54.2% Dutch: 94.6% SEB Good 
Hornman 201790  The Netherlands 32–36 wk: 644; 38–41 wk: 375 4 y 2002–2003 34–36 wk: 58.3%; 38–41 wk: 47.1% Non-Dutch background 4.3% DD Good 
Hua 202191  China 32–33 wk: 2322; 34–36 wk: 12 915; 37–38 wk: 38 875; 39–40 wk: 76 501 3–5 y 2013–2016 32–33 wk: 54.0%; 34–36 wk: 55.3%; 37–38 wk: 55.3%; 39–40 wk: 51.6% Han Chinese 99% Motor Good 
You 2019a92  China 34–36 wk: 102; 37–42 wk: 153 24–30 mo 2011–2013 34–36 wk: 66.7%; 37–42 wk: 61.4% NR DD, CP Good 
You 2019b93  China 34–36 wk: 112; 37–42 wk: 179 2 y 2013–2015 34–36 wk: 54.5%; 37–42 wk: 47.5% NR DD, CP Fair 
Zhou 202045  China 32–37 wk: 83; 37–42 wk:1665 1 mo–5 y 2011–2016 32–37 wk: 51.8%; 37–42 wk: 53.8% NR DD Good 
Stene-Larsen 201494  Norway 34–36 wk: 1673; 37–38 wk: 7109; 39–41 wk: 30 641 3 y 1999–2008 34–36 wk: 51.3%; 37–38 wk: 48.6%; 39–41 wk: 50.4% NR DD Good 
Strand 201395  Norway 32–36 wk: 23 763; 37–40 wk: 522 551 4 y (minimum) 1996–2006 NR - only reported for pre-eclampsia versus no pre-eclampsia NR CP Good 
Ask 201896  Norway 34–36 wk: 1755; 37–38 wk: 6732; 40 wk: 11 753 (SEB) 5 y 1999–2008 Whole cohort 51.3% (NR by gestation) NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Zambrana 202197  Norway Total: 26 769 3 y 1999–2008 51.5% (NR by gestation) NR DD Good 
Demestre 201698  Spain 34–36 wk: 90; 38–41 wk: 89 4 y 2009 34–36 wk: 61%; 38–41 wk: 55% NR DD Good 
Oros 201499  Spain 34–36 wk: 6; ≥37 wk: 96 6–13 y 1997–2005 34–36 wk: 40.9%; term 52.6% Caucasian 99.3% Cognition Fair 
Pérez-Pereira 2014100  Spain 34–36 wk: 47; ≥37 wk: 36 2.5 y Unclear 34–36 wk: 56.5%; ≥37 wk: 51.4% NR Speech Fair 
Pérez-Pereira 2020101  Spain 32–33 wk: 31; 34–36 wk: 42; ≥37 wk: 33 5 y Unclear 52% NR Cognition Good 
Klassen 2004102  Canada 33–37 wk: 341; >37 wk: 259 (vision) 3.5 y 1996–1997 33–37 wk: 45.2%; >37 wk: 40.2% NR DD, hearing, vision Fair 
Leavey 2013103  Canada 32–33 wk: 2373; 34–36 wk: 13 108; 37–38 wk: 51 307; 39–41 wk: 146 467 Minimum 3 y 1998–2004 49% NR ASD Good 
Faleschini 2020104  Canada 32–36 wk: 89; ≥37 wk: 1841 4 y 1997–1998 32–36 wk: 52%; ≥37 wk: 50% Born in Canada 87% SEB, ADHD Good 
Chen 2020105  Sweden 37–38 wk: 132 997; 39–41 wk: 530 988 3 y 1998–2009 51.1% NR Cognition Good 
Beer 2022106  Sweden 32–36 wk: 47 859; 37–41 wk: 1 071 729 3–15 y 2002–2014 NR NR ADHD Good 
Chen 2022107  Sweden 32–36 wk: 77 986; 37–38 wk: 346 859; 39–40 wk: 965 756 0–16 y (median 9.4 y) 1998–2016 50.4% (NR by gestation) NR CP Good 
Larsen 2021108  Denmark 32–33 wk: 6067; 34–36 wk: 29 821; 37–41 wk: 531 996 (SEN) 4–6 y 1997–2013 NR - it is only reported for each disorder not each gestation NR SEN, CP Good 
Zhu 2012109  Denmark 32–33 wk: 123; 34–36 wk: 721; 37–38 wk: 3519; 39–40 wk: 11 743 7 y 1996–2002 51% NR Motor Good 
Larroque 2008110  France 32 wk: 484; 39–40 wk: 389 (vision) 5 y 1997 52% male NR Cognition, hearing, vision, CP Good 
Bailhache 2022111  France 32–36 wk: 421; ≥37 wk: 4164 9 y 2011 50.9% NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Voigt 2012112  Germany 32–37 wk: 88; ≥38 wk: 86 2 y 2007–2008 32–37 wk 51%; ≥38 wk 47% NR Cognition, SEB, ADHD Fair 
Reuner 2015113  Germany 33–36 wk 54; ≥37 wk 38 2 y 2008–2009 33–36 wk: 50%; ≥37 wk: 47% NR Cognition Fair 
Darlow 2009114  New Zealand 33–36 wk: 112; ≥37 wk: 101 2 y 2001–2002 33–36 wk: 61%; term admissions 59% Māori 5.9% CP, DD Good 
Berry 2018115  New Zealand 33–36 wk: 19 089; 37–38 wk: 70 026; 39–40 wk:180 987 (vision) 6–11 y 1998–2000 and 2005–2015 51.4% European 67%; Pacific Islander 12%; Māori 21% SEN, education aged 12–17 y, hearing, vision Good 
Yang 2010116  Belarus 38 wk: 2100; 39–41 wk: 11 074 6 y 1996–1997 38 wk: 53.8%; 39 wk: 50.8%; 40 wk: 51.7% NR Cognition Good 
Polic 2017117  Croatia 34–36 wk: 126; 37–40 wk: 131 6–12 y 2002–2008 34–36 wk: 56.3%; 37–40 wk: 59.5% NR SEB Good 
Hirvonen 2015118  Finland 32–33 wk: 4862; 34–36 wk: 28 152; ≥37 wk: 671 988 7 y 1996–2008 32–33 wk: 54.9%; 34–36 wk: 54.2%; ≥37 wk: 50.8% NR CP Good 
Drougia 2007119  Greece 34–36 wk 594; ≥37 wk: 1719 Minimum 2 y 1997–2003 NR NR CP Good 
Tso 2022120  Hong Kong 33–36 wk: 22 811; 37–41 wk: 330 087 6–17 y, mean 11 y 7 mo 2004–2014 52% NR ADHD Good 
Patil 2014121  India 32–36 wk: 100; ≥37 wk: 100 4–6 y unclear Preterm 59%; term 61% NR DD Fair 
Marotta 2020122  Italy 34–36 wk: 53; ≥37 wk: 53 5–11 y (mean 8) 2002–2010 39.6% NR SEB Fair 
Higa Diez 2016123  Japan 34–36 wk: 645; 37–38 wk: 5103; 39–41 wk: 11752 8 y 2001 34–36 wk 60.1%; 37–38 wk 55.3%; 39–41 wk 49.7% NR SEB, ADHD Good 
Al-Haddad 2017124  Lebanon 32–36 wk: 51; ≥ 37 wk: 69 4.9 y (mean) 2006–2014 55% NR Vision Fair 

ASD, autism spectrum disorder; ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; CP, cerebral palsy; DD, developmental disorder; NA, not applicable; NR, not recorded; SEB, social, emotional, and behavioral disorders; SEN, special educational needs.

TABLE 2

Summary

Developmental Disorder32–33 wk32–36 wk34–36 wk37–38 wk
CP relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
CP prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
DCD relative risk ↔ a ↑ ↑ 
DCD prevalence ↑ a ↑ ↑ 
Visual impairment relative risk ↔ ↔ a ↑ 
Visual impairment prevalence ↔ ↑ a ↑ 
Hearing impairment relative risk ↔ ↔ a ↑ 
Hearing impairment prevalence ↓ ↑ a ↑ 
Sleep apnea relative risk a ↑ a a 
Sleep apnea prevalence a ↑ a a 
SEB global relative risk ↔ b ↑ ↔ 
SEB global prevalence ↔ b ↑ ↔ 
SEB global (continuous) a ↓ ↔ a 
SEB internalizing relative risk a ↔ ↔ a 
SEB internalizing prevalence a ↔ ↑ a 
SEB externalizing relative risk a ↔ a a 
SEB externalizing prevalence a ↔ a a 
SEB social relative risk ↑ b ↑ a 
SEB social prevalence ↔ b ↑ a 
ADHD relative risk a ↑ ↑ ↑ 
ADHD prevalence a ↑ ↑ ↑ 
ADHD (continuous) a  ↑ ↔ 
ASD relative risk ↑ a ↑ ↔ 
ASD prevalence  a ↑ ↓ 
Global DD relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Global DD prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Language DD relative risk a ↔ ↔ ↔ 
Language DD prevalence a ↑ ↑ ↑ 
Motor DD relative risk a ↔ ↔ a 
Motor DD prevalence a ↑ ↔ a 
Cognitive DD relative risk a ↑ ↑ a 
Cognitive DD prevalence a ↔ ↑ a 
Cognitive impairment relative risk ↑ a ↔ ↑ 
Cognitive impairment prevalence ↑ a ↑ ↑ 
Cognitive impairment (continuous) ↔ b ↑ ↑ 
Executive function (continuous) a ↔ ↔ a 
Not school ready relative risk ↔ b ↑ ↑ 
Not school ready prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 6–11 y relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 6–11 y prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 12–17 y relative risk ↔ b ↔ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 12–17 y prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↔ 
Special educational needs relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Special educational needs prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Developmental Disorder32–33 wk32–36 wk34–36 wk37–38 wk
CP relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
CP prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
DCD relative risk ↔ a ↑ ↑ 
DCD prevalence ↑ a ↑ ↑ 
Visual impairment relative risk ↔ ↔ a ↑ 
Visual impairment prevalence ↔ ↑ a ↑ 
Hearing impairment relative risk ↔ ↔ a ↑ 
Hearing impairment prevalence ↓ ↑ a ↑ 
Sleep apnea relative risk a ↑ a a 
Sleep apnea prevalence a ↑ a a 
SEB global relative risk ↔ b ↑ ↔ 
SEB global prevalence ↔ b ↑ ↔ 
SEB global (continuous) a ↓ ↔ a 
SEB internalizing relative risk a ↔ ↔ a 
SEB internalizing prevalence a ↔ ↑ a 
SEB externalizing relative risk a ↔ a a 
SEB externalizing prevalence a ↔ a a 
SEB social relative risk ↑ b ↑ a 
SEB social prevalence ↔ b ↑ a 
ADHD relative risk a ↑ ↑ ↑ 
ADHD prevalence a ↑ ↑ ↑ 
ADHD (continuous) a  ↑ ↔ 
ASD relative risk ↑ a ↑ ↔ 
ASD prevalence  a ↑ ↓ 
Global DD relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Global DD prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Language DD relative risk a ↔ ↔ ↔ 
Language DD prevalence a ↑ ↑ ↑ 
Motor DD relative risk a ↔ ↔ a 
Motor DD prevalence a ↑ ↔ a 
Cognitive DD relative risk a ↑ ↑ a 
Cognitive DD prevalence a ↔ ↑ a 
Cognitive impairment relative risk ↑ a ↔ ↑ 
Cognitive impairment prevalence ↑ a ↑ ↑ 
Cognitive impairment (continuous) ↔ b ↑ ↑ 
Executive function (continuous) a ↔ ↔ a 
Not school ready relative risk ↔ b ↑ ↑ 
Not school ready prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 6–11 y relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 6–11 y prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 12–17 y relative risk ↔ b ↔ ↑ 
Low educational achievement 12–17 y prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↔ 
Special educational needs relative risk ↑ b ↑ ↑ 
Special educational needs prevalence ↑ b ↑ ↑ 

↑, increased RR with CI not crossing “1”, or increased prevalence compared with term, with CI not overlapping with term; ↓, reduced RR with CI not crossing “1”, or decreased prevalence compared with term, with CI not overlapping with term; ↔, difference not statistically significant. ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; DD, developmental disorder; SEB, social, emotional, and behavioral disorders.

a

No data.

b

Not presented (data available for both 3233 wk and 3436 wk groups).

The relative risk of screening positive for, or a diagnosis of, a developmental disorder compared with children born at term, was highest in the moderately preterm group; there was a statistically significant increased relative risk of CP, social problems, ASD, global DD, cognitive impairment, low educational attainment, and special educational needs (Figs 29). Forest plots for the remaining disorders are presented in the supplemental information (Supplemental Figs 1237).

FIGURE 2

Relative risk of cerebral palsy by gestational age.

FIGURE 2

Relative risk of cerebral palsy by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 3

Prevalence of cerebral palsy by gestational age.

FIGURE 3

Prevalence of cerebral palsy by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 4

Relative risk of global developmental delay by gestational age.

FIGURE 4

Relative risk of global developmental delay by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 5

Prevalence of global developmental delay by gestational age.

FIGURE 5

Prevalence of global developmental delay by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 6

Relative risk of ADHD or ADHD symptoms by gestational age.

FIGURE 6

Relative risk of ADHD or ADHD symptoms by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 7

Prevalence of ADHD or ADHD symptoms by gestational age.

FIGURE 7

Prevalence of ADHD or ADHD symptoms by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 8

Relative risk of low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years by gestational age.

FIGURE 8

Relative risk of low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years by gestational age.

Close modal
FIGURE 9

Prevalence of low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years by gestational age.

FIGURE 9

Prevalence of low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years by gestational age.

Close modal

The relative risk of CP compared with children born at term was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 14.1 (95% confidence intervals [CI]: 12.3–16.0), 34 to 36 weeks: 3.52 (95% CI: 3.16–3.92) and 37 to 38 weeks: 1.44 (95% CI: 1.32–1.58). The prevalence of CP per 1000 children was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 17.1 (95% CI: 15.1–19.3), 34 to 36 weeks: 2.95 (95% CI: 2.53–3.39), 37 to 38 weeks: 2.05 (95% CI: 1.91–2.21), and ≥37 weeks: 0.53 (95% CI: 0.50–0.57).

The relative risk of global developmental delay compared with children born at term was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 2.89 (95% CI: 2.77–3.02), 34 to 36 weeks: 1.61 (95% CI: 1.25–2.08) and 37 to 38 weeks: 1.14 (95% CI: 1.12–1.16). The prevalence of global developmental delay per 1000 children was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 350 (95% CI: 335–365), 34 to 36 weeks: 132 (95% CI: 128–136), 37 to 38 weeks: 138 (95% CI: 136–140), and ≥37 weeks: 65.5 (95% CI: 64.7–66.3).

The relative risk of ADHD compared with children born at term was, for 32 to 36 weeks: 1.25 (95% CI: 1.14–1.38), 34 to 36 weeks: 1.62 (95% CI: 1.38–1.90), 37 to 38 weeks: 1.19 (95% CI: 1.00–1.42). The prevalence of ADHD per 1000 children was, for 32 to 36 weeks: 34.9 (95% CI: 33.6–36.3), 34 to 36 weeks: 75.7 (95% CI: 66.1–86.0), 37 to 38 weeks: 37.2 (95% CI: 32.4–42.8), and ≥37 weeks 26.6 (95% CI: 26.3–26.8).

The relative risk of low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years compared with children born at term was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 1.96 (95% CI: 1.11–3.43), 34 to 36 weeks: 1.21 (95% CI: 1.10–1.32), and 37 to 38 weeks: 1.13 (95% CI: 1.08–1.19). The prevalence of low educational achievement aged 6–11 years per 1000 children was, for 32 to 33 weeks: 304 (95% CI: 285–324), 34 to 36 weeks: 199 (95% CI: 195–203), 37 to 38 weeks: 224 (95% CI: 221–227), and ≥37 weeks: 163 (95% CI: 162–164). A similar pattern is seen across the other developmental disorders, as shown in Supplemental Figs 12–39 (in the supplemental information) and in the prevalence summary bar charts (Fig 10).

FIGURE 10

Bar chart showing pooled prevalence per 1000 children of developmental disorder by gestational group with 95% confidence intervals.

FIGURE 10

Bar chart showing pooled prevalence per 1000 children of developmental disorder by gestational group with 95% confidence intervals.

Close modal

Visual or hearing impairment were slightly more prevalent in some of the groups born before full term. The relative risk of visual or hearing impairment compared with children born at term was increased in all gestational subgroups, although this only reached statistical significance in the 37 to 38 week gestation group (Supplemental Figs 1518). There was a small increased risk and prevalence of sleep apnea but its prevalence is low compared with other disorders (Fig 10).

There is an association with reduced gestational age and increased risk and prevalence of all forms of social, emotional, and behavioral problems, except externalizing disorders. The most prevalent problem of this type is social problems (eg, showing empathy and playing with other children28 ). There was a small increased risk and prevalence of ASD in the 34 to 36 week group. The only disorder found with neither an increased relative risk nor prevalence among the 32 to 36 week children was externalizing behaviors; this was based on 1 study, as shown in Table 3. There was no increased relative risk of executive function disorders (there is no prevalence estimation since it was reported as a continuous outcome).

TABLE 3

Summary Describing the Relative Risk and the Number of Studies and Participants Per Gestational Age Subgroup Per Outcome. Binary Outcomes Unless Specified

Developmental Disorder32–33 wk32–36 wk34–36 wk37–38 wkTermTotal
CP 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 14.1 (12.3 to 16.0) NP 3.52 (3.16 to 3.92) 1.44 (1.32 to 1.58)   
 Number of children 15 365 NP 87 053 346 859 2 695 512 3 144 789 
 Number of studies NP 
DCD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 2.43 (0.82 to 7.25) ND 1.65 (1.10 to 2.46) 1.15 (1.06 to 1.24)   
 Number of children 2445 ND 13 636 42 394 88 244 146 719 
 Number of studies ND 
Visual impairment 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 7.23 (0.92 to 56.9) 1.42 (0.94 to 2.15) ND 1.26 (1.20 to 1.32)   
 Number of children 484 19 596 ND 70 026 183 047 273 153 
 Number of studies ND 
Hearing impairment 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 2.32 (0.09 to 56.7) 1.03 (0.35 to 3.04) ND 1.19 (1.13 to 1.25)   
 Number of children 503 19 445 ND 70 053 181 688 271 689 
 Number of studies ND 
Sleep apnea 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.30 (1.15 to 1.46) ND ND   
 Number of children ND 22 039 ND ND 377 952 399 991 
 Number of studies ND ND ND 
SEB global (binary) 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.12 (0.77 to 1.62) ND 1.24 (1.04 to 1.48) 1.11 (0.94 to 1.32)   
 Number of children 248 ND 1232 2864 10 571 14 915 
 Number of studies ND 
SEB global (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) ND 0.41 (0.23 to 0.59) 0.80 (0.54 to 1.05) ND   
 Number of children ND 377 126 ND 313 816 
 Number of studies ND ND 
SEB internalizing 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.22 (0.89 to 1.69) 0.82 (0.37 to 1.81) ND   
 Number of children ND 597 53 ND 4368 501 
 Number of studies ND ND 
SEB externalizing 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 2.29 (0.62 to 8.47) NA ND   
 Number of children ND 176 NA ND 151 327 
 Number of studies ND NA ND 
SEB social 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.51 (1.03 to 2.20) NP 1.60 (1.04 to 2.46) ND   
 Number of children 84 NP 594 ND 813 1491 
 Number of studies NP ND 
ADHD (binary) 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.25 (1.14 to 1.38) 1.62 (1.38 to 1.90) 1.19 (1.00 to 1.42)   
 Number of children ND 71 09 2733 5103 1 443 744 1 522 671 
 Number of studies ND 
ADHD (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) ND 0.15 (−0.04 to 0.34) 0.10 (0.06 to 0.15) 0.11 (−0.07 to 0.29)   
 Number of children ND 525 1907 7132 15 494 25 058 
 Number of studies ND 
ASD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.75 (1.13 to 2.69) ND 1.42 (1.08 to 1.88) 1.13 (0.98 to 1.29)   
 Number of children 2455 ND 25 592 51 307 324 345 403 699 
 Number of studies ND 
Global DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 2.89 (2.77 to 3.02) NP 1.61 (1.25 to 2.08) 1.14 (1.12 to 1.16)   
 Number of children 3973 NP 30 266 79 228 358 670 472 137 
 Number of studies NP 
Language DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 2.15 (0.98 to 4.70) 1.79 (0.95 to 3.36) 1.53 (0.95, 2.47)   
 Number of children ND 745 1673a 7109a 31 272a 40 799a 
 Number of studies ND 
Motor DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.69 (0.99 to 2.88) 4.26 (0.53 to 34.4) ND   
 Number of children ND 522 1414 ND 7108 9044 
 Number of studies ND ND 
Cognitive DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 2.32 (1.08 to 4.99) 1.29 (1.14 to 1.46) ND   
 Number of children ND 525 1200 ND 6780 8505 
 Number of studies ND ND 
Cognitive impairment (binary) 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.86 (1.39 to 2.48) ND 1.86 (0.93 to 3.72) 1.16 (1.07 to 1.27)   
 Number of children 631 ND 1315 135 185 540 350 677 481 
 Number of studies ND 
Cognitive impairment (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) −0.07 (−0.56 to 0.41) NP −0.31 (−0.54 to −0.08) −0.60 (−1.15 to −0.05)   
 Number of children 31 NP 564 469 11 529 12 593 
 Number of studies NP 
Executive function (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) ND 0.33 (−0.08 to 0.74) −0.06 (−0.49 to 0.37) ND   
 Number of children ND 45 39 ND 90 174 
 Number of studies ND ND 
Not school ready relative risk 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.28 (0.94 to 1.76) NP 1.31 (1.23 to 1.39) 1.11 (1.08, 1.14)   
 Number of children 199 NP 14 485 29 238 239 737 283 659 
 Number of studies NP 
Low educational achievement 6–11 yrs 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.96 (1.11 to 3.43) NP 1.21 (1.10 to 1.32) 1.13 (1.08, 1.19)   
 Number of children 2215 NP 40 586 60 995 472 354 576 150 
 Number of studies NP 
Low educational achievement 12–17 yrs 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.04 (0.82 to 1.33) NP 1.08 (0.97 to 1.21) 1.03 (1.01 to 1.05)   
 Number of children 76 NP 401 28 948 83 073 112 498 
 Number of studies NP 
Special educational needs 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.60 (1.25 to 2.04) NP 1.36 (1.23 to 1.51) 1.17 (1.09 to 1.26)   
 Number of children 8429 NP 53 178 129 778 1 053 459 1 244 844 
 Number of studies NP 
Developmental Disorder32–33 wk32–36 wk34–36 wk37–38 wkTermTotal
CP 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 14.1 (12.3 to 16.0) NP 3.52 (3.16 to 3.92) 1.44 (1.32 to 1.58)   
 Number of children 15 365 NP 87 053 346 859 2 695 512 3 144 789 
 Number of studies NP 
DCD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 2.43 (0.82 to 7.25) ND 1.65 (1.10 to 2.46) 1.15 (1.06 to 1.24)   
 Number of children 2445 ND 13 636 42 394 88 244 146 719 
 Number of studies ND 
Visual impairment 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 7.23 (0.92 to 56.9) 1.42 (0.94 to 2.15) ND 1.26 (1.20 to 1.32)   
 Number of children 484 19 596 ND 70 026 183 047 273 153 
 Number of studies ND 
Hearing impairment 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 2.32 (0.09 to 56.7) 1.03 (0.35 to 3.04) ND 1.19 (1.13 to 1.25)   
 Number of children 503 19 445 ND 70 053 181 688 271 689 
 Number of studies ND 
Sleep apnea 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.30 (1.15 to 1.46) ND ND   
 Number of children ND 22 039 ND ND 377 952 399 991 
 Number of studies ND ND ND 
SEB global (binary) 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.12 (0.77 to 1.62) ND 1.24 (1.04 to 1.48) 1.11 (0.94 to 1.32)   
 Number of children 248 ND 1232 2864 10 571 14 915 
 Number of studies ND 
SEB global (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) ND 0.41 (0.23 to 0.59) 0.80 (0.54 to 1.05) ND   
 Number of children ND 377 126 ND 313 816 
 Number of studies ND ND 
SEB internalizing 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.22 (0.89 to 1.69) 0.82 (0.37 to 1.81) ND   
 Number of children ND 597 53 ND 4368 501 
 Number of studies ND ND 
SEB externalizing 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 2.29 (0.62 to 8.47) NA ND   
 Number of children ND 176 NA ND 151 327 
 Number of studies ND NA ND 
SEB social 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.51 (1.03 to 2.20) NP 1.60 (1.04 to 2.46) ND   
 Number of children 84 NP 594 ND 813 1491 
 Number of studies NP ND 
ADHD (binary) 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.25 (1.14 to 1.38) 1.62 (1.38 to 1.90) 1.19 (1.00 to 1.42)   
 Number of children ND 71 09 2733 5103 1 443 744 1 522 671 
 Number of studies ND 
ADHD (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) ND 0.15 (−0.04 to 0.34) 0.10 (0.06 to 0.15) 0.11 (−0.07 to 0.29)   
 Number of children ND 525 1907 7132 15 494 25 058 
 Number of studies ND 
ASD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.75 (1.13 to 2.69) ND 1.42 (1.08 to 1.88) 1.13 (0.98 to 1.29)   
 Number of children 2455 ND 25 592 51 307 324 345 403 699 
 Number of studies ND 
Global DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 2.89 (2.77 to 3.02) NP 1.61 (1.25 to 2.08) 1.14 (1.12 to 1.16)   
 Number of children 3973 NP 30 266 79 228 358 670 472 137 
 Number of studies NP 
Language DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 2.15 (0.98 to 4.70) 1.79 (0.95 to 3.36) 1.53 (0.95, 2.47)   
 Number of children ND 745 1673a 7109a 31 272a 40 799a 
 Number of studies ND 
Motor DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 1.69 (0.99 to 2.88) 4.26 (0.53 to 34.4) ND   
 Number of children ND 522 1414 ND 7108 9044 
 Number of studies ND ND 
Cognitive DD 
 Relative risk (95% CI) ND 2.32 (1.08 to 4.99) 1.29 (1.14 to 1.46) ND   
 Number of children ND 525 1200 ND 6780 8505 
 Number of studies ND ND 
Cognitive impairment (binary) 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.86 (1.39 to 2.48) ND 1.86 (0.93 to 3.72) 1.16 (1.07 to 1.27)   
 Number of children 631 ND 1315 135 185 540 350 677 481 
 Number of studies ND 
Cognitive impairment (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) −0.07 (−0.56 to 0.41) NP −0.31 (−0.54 to −0.08) −0.60 (−1.15 to −0.05)   
 Number of children 31 NP 564 469 11 529 12 593 
 Number of studies NP 
Executive function (continuous) 
 Standardized mean difference (95% CI) ND 0.33 (−0.08 to 0.74) −0.06 (−0.49 to 0.37) ND   
 Number of children ND 45 39 ND 90 174 
 Number of studies ND ND 
Not school ready relative risk 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.28 (0.94 to 1.76) NP 1.31 (1.23 to 1.39) 1.11 (1.08, 1.14)   
 Number of children 199 NP 14 485 29 238 239 737 283 659 
 Number of studies NP 
Low educational achievement 6–11 yrs 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.96 (1.11 to 3.43) NP 1.21 (1.10 to 1.32) 1.13 (1.08, 1.19)   
 Number of children 2215 NP 40 586 60 995 472 354 576 150 
 Number of studies NP 
Low educational achievement 12–17 yrs 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.04 (0.82 to 1.33) NP 1.08 (0.97 to 1.21) 1.03 (1.01 to 1.05)   
 Number of children 76 NP 401 28 948 83 073 112 498 
 Number of studies NP 
Special educational needs 
 Relative risk (95% CI) 1.60 (1.25 to 2.04) NP 1.36 (1.23 to 1.51) 1.17 (1.09 to 1.26)   
 Number of children 8429 NP 53 178 129 778 1 053 459 1 244 844 
 Number of studies NP 

ND, data; NP, not presented (data available for both 32–33 wk and 34–36 wk groups).

a

This does not include Zambrana 2021, as only the results of the regression were presented, therefore the raw numbers could not be extracted.

Among early term children, prevalence and relative risk of developmental disorder was increased for several disorders: CP, DCD, visual impairment, ADHD, global developmental delay, cognitive impairment, not school ready, low educational achievement, and special education needs (Table 2). There was no data for early term children for: sleep apnea, internalizing behaviors, externalizing behaviors, social problems, motor developmental delay, or cognitive developmental delay.

When only the papers with a full term (39–40/41 weeks) comparison group were included, only 1 paper remained in each gestational subgroup for CP and global developmental delay. The overall interpretation was unchanged in all cases (Supplemental Figs 7073).

In general, the heterogeneity statistics (I2) were 0 (Figs 29). Some subgroups had a higher heterogeneity, for example in the educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years meta-analysis (Fig 8). A possible source of heterogeneity was different measurements for developmental disorder; in the CP meta-analysis Larroque 2008, You 2019a, and You 2019b examined participants rather than using linked records, possibly explaining why these studies had a higher prevalence of CP. The different methods used to identify each developmental disorder are shown in Supplemental Table 8. Heterogeneity between subgroups was low.

The majority of funnel plots (16 of 24) were symmetrical with nonsignificant Egger’s tests (14 of 24) (Supplemental Table 9 and Supplemental Figs 4869 in the supplemental information).

Children born between 32 and 38 weeks are at increased risk of screening positive for, or receiving a diagnosis of, a developmental disorder compared with children born at term. In most cases an inverse gradient association with gestational age was demonstrated. The highest increased relative risk compared with children born at term was for children born 32 to 33 weeks for CP, but CP has a low prevalence compared with other developmental disorders.

The association between increased risk and prevalence of global developmental delay or language delay compared with children born at term is evident in all gestational age groups between 32 and 38 weeks. Interpreting the cognitive developmental delay meta-analysis is challenging; prevalence was relatively high in the term group, with confidence intervals overlapping the 32 and 36 week group. However, there was an increased relative risk of cognitive developmental delay in the 32 to 36 week group compared with term. It is likely that the picture seen with prevalence is a result of which studies reported different gestational age groups.

In 2 disorders (hearing impairment and ASD), 1 group born 32 to 38 weeks had a lower prevalence of developmental disorder than the term group (Table 2). In both cases this was where the gestational subgroup only included results from 1 study (Table 3). It is likely that this is a peculiarity of the tools used to assess the disorder in that study, since when the relative risks are considered, this effect was not seen.

These findings are largely consistent with previous research that has shown increased risk of developmental disorders among children born moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term.3,8,10,19  This review demonstrates that difficulties faced by children born 32 to 38 weeks persist through childhood, with evidence of increased risk and prevalence of cognitive impairment and low educational achievement aged 6 to 11 years, in contrast to previous research suggesting developmental delay in preterm infants may be transient.29,30 

The proportion of children affected by a developmental disorder is generally lower among children born between 32 to 38 weeks compared with extremely preterm children. However, late preterm and early term birth are common; in the United States in 2020, 7.4% of children were born late preterm, whereas only 2.7% were born under 34 weeks.11  Therefore, small increases in relative risk (compared with full term children) may have a considerable affect, both clinically and economically, at a population level.3033 

Developmental disorders affect 35% to 52% of children born extremely or very preterm31,34  (which is not substantially higher than the prevalence of DCD, social problems, and low educational achievement found in children born 32–38 weeks in this meta-analysis). CP affects 8% to 9% of very preterm children,29  compared with 1.71% (95% CI: 1.51–1.93) born between 32 and 33 weeks in this review. Children born between 32 and 38 weeks may experience a different profile of disorders to extremely preterm children, possibly mediated through different pathways.13,29,33,35  Birth before full term may be the result of maternal ill health (eg, preeclampsia, gestational diabetes, infection) and a suboptimal intrauterine environment.33  These antenatal issues may be driving the increased risk and prevalence of developmental disorders, as opposed to the prematurity per se.7,11,36  Conversely, babies born just a few weeks early have markedly different brain maturation to full term children.10,35,37  It is possible that birth between 32 and 38 weeks’ gestation may disrupt evolution of neural connections, potentially resulting in developmental disorder.19,33  Children born before full term are more likely to have medical complications in the immediate neonatal period, in some cases leading directly to a developmental disorder.19,38  After the neonatal period, children born late preterm are 2 to 3 times more likely to attend the emergency department or be admitted to hospital.6,39  The increased medical needs of children born before full term affect both the child and family. Parents of late preterm infants have been shown to have high emotional distress and anxiety levels.6,40  Furthermore, admission to the neonatal unit is associated with both acute stress and post-traumatic stress disorder among parents.41  It is plausible that early complications, prolonged admission, or readmission to hospital indirectly affects child development via the negative effect on the whole family.

This was a broad ranging, comprehensive review. The search strategy identified a large number of studies; there was a total of over 8 million children in the meta-analyses. Including a full range of developmental disorders enabled comparison of prevalence of different developmental disorders across gestational ages. Calculating prevalence meant the increased risk could be contextualized. Although there were a large number of children in total, because each developmental disorder was considered separately according to gestational age subgroups, there were sometimes small numbers in each subgroup (Table 3). There were relatively few studies reporting outcomes for early term children; the subgroup meta-analysis often only contained data from 1 study, although this often represented more children than in the other gestational subgroups combined.

Developmental disorders are, by definition, a heterogeneous group. To determine which conditions to investigate and maximize this review’s applicability, we used the NICE guideline, “Developmental follow-up of children and young people born preterm,” as a reference.18  The NICE guideline considers “problems with inattention, impulsivity, or hyperactivity” separately from “executive function problems.” However, ADHD is closely associated with impaired executive function, and some have argued that “executive function problems” do not represent a diagnosis as such.42,43  The NICE guidance also describes increased risk of low educational achievement (“educational attainment” in the UK) among children born preterm. Although there are probably many children who have low educational achievement but do not have a diagnosed developmental disorder, low educational achievement has been demonstrated to be associated with early developmental difficulties.44  Including educational achievement also gives the opportunity to examine outcomes at a later stage of childhood, demonstrating that the association between birth before full term and developmental disorder can persist into adolescence.

The tools measuring outcomes were numerous and varied, as described in Supplemental Table 8, which likely accounts for some of the different prevalence (eg, DCD Supplemental Fig 13). Furthermore, although some studies used diagnostic codes in medical records to determine their outcome, many studies used questionnaires, tests, and tools (eg, the Ages and Stages Questionnaire) that were not developed to make formal diagnoses, but rather were intended as screening tools for developmental problems.45  Thus, when considering the results of this review, it is crucial to bear in mind that although some children may screen positive for a potential developmental disorder or be highlighted as a “cause for concern,” that does not equate to a formal diagnosis of a specific developmental disorder.

As with other reviews,10  the children were assessed at different ages and the term comparison groups were variable, for example ≥37 weeks, >37 weeks, ≥39 weeks, 39 to 41 weeks, or 40 weeks. Comparator groups were (where possible) children born at full term (39–40/41 weeks), or if this data were not presented, at term (≥37 weeks). This could account to some extent for why the prevalence of some disorders (eg, not school ready, low educational achievement 6–11 years) is lower in the 37 to 38 week group than in the term group. Although it would have been preferable to have a homogenous term comparator group, only including studies that used a full term (39–40/41 weeks) comparison group would have resulted in a substantial loss of data; of the 76 included studies, only 24 used a full term comparison group. A sensitivity analysis was undertaken for CP, global developmental delay, educational outcomes aged 6 to 11 years, and ADHD, where only the papers with a full term comparison group were included (Supplemental Figs 7073). In some cases, this resulted in only 1 study in each gestational age subgroup. The overall interpretation was unchanged.

Some studies used atypical cohorts, eg, Drougia 2007, Klassen 2004, and Polic 2017 only included children who admitted to a neonatal unit; in these cases, their term comparator groups would not represent typical term-born children.

The majority of studies were from economically developed countries and all were published in English, possibly limiting generalizability. Outside of the United States, there was limited data on children with non-European heritage; this is important, as race or ethnicity may impact the likelihood of screening positive for, or receiving a diagnosis of, a developmental disorder.3,18,46 

Understanding the long-term implications of birth before full term when balanced against short term risk to the mother and fetus may influence obstetric decision making.8  It is vital that all healthcare professionals, particularly pediatricians, are well informed of the potential consequences of preterm birth in order that they can give evidence based information to families and so opportunities for early intervention are not missed. Children born at 32 to 38 weeks may benefit from increased monitoring of their development, but most neonatal follow-up programs only apply to children born very preterm, in line with American Academy of Pediatrics guidelines.6,32,47  However, it is likely that, especially among the more mature gestations, many children will not have any developmental disorders.31  Depending on the structure and financing of the healthcare systems, routine appointments for all these children may be impractical and undesirable. A more effective approach would be collaborating with the education sector (which currently bears the majority of the cost associated with prematurity48 ). Teachers should be informed if they have students who are born preterm and early term and receive training on how to support them.8,49  It is also likely that early childhood risks for poorer outcomes are additive; determining which groups of children born 32 to 38 weeks are most at risk for developmental disorder, selectively following them up, and providing family support would be a pragmatic approach.6  Empowering parents with information on developmental risks is important, not only for the early recognition of problems, but also to give parents agency.47  It is currently unclear which children born between 32 and 38 weeks are at the highest risk32  or to what extent early interventions shown to benefit very preterm children might also benefit children born 32 to 38 weeks.12,32,4852 

We have demonstrated that for many gestational subgroups the research into developmental disorders is sparse (Table 3) and gaps in the empirical knowledge persist. In future research, consistent outcome measurements, full term control groups (39–40/41 weeks) and standardized gestational age groups should be used, to allow easier comparison.8,19,32 

This review has found evidence of an inverse relationship between birth before full term and the majority of developmental disorders. Low educational achievement, DCD, global developmental delay, and cognitive impairment were the most prevalent disorders among children born 32 to 38 weeks. Some of the increased risks are small but may have significant consequences both clinically and economically at a population level, as birth between 32 and 38 weeks is common. Future research should focus on determining which subgroups of children born 32 to 38 weeks are at particularly high risk and how these children can be supported to reach their potential.

Thanks to David Brown, Academic Liaison Librarian for Health Sciences, for help with the search strategy design.

Dr Pettinger conceptualized and designed the study, performed the literature search, data extraction and data analysis, drafted the initial manuscript, and revised the manuscript; Mrs Copper participated in the literature search and data extraction and critically reviewed the manuscript; Drs Blower, Boyle, Hewitt, and Fraser supervised the study design, the literature search, data extraction and analysis, and critically reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted and agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

FUNDING: Dr Pettinger, Doctoral Research Fellow, is funded by the National Institute for Health and Social Care Research (NIHR) for this research (award ref. NIHR301738). The funder has no role in the interpretation of data, writing of the report, or decision to submit for publication. The views expressed in this publication are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR, NHS, or the UK Department of Health and Social Care.

CONFLICT OF INTEREST DISCLOSURES: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

ADHD

attention deficit hyperactivity disorder

ASD

autism spectrum disorder

CI

confidence interval

CP

cerebral palsy

DCD

developmental coordination disorder

1
Shevell
MI
.
Present conceptualization of early childhood neurodevelopmental disabilities
.
J Child Neurol
.
2010
;
25
(
1
):
120
126
2
McGowan
JE
,
Alderdice
FA
,
Holmes
VA
,
Johnston
L
.
Early childhood development of late-preterm infants: a systematic review
.
Pediatrics
.
2011
;
127
(
6
):
1111
1124
3
Johnson
S
,
Evans
TA
,
Draper
ES
, et al
.
Neurodevelopmental outcomes following late and moderate prematurity: a population-based cohort study
.
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed
.
2015
;
100
(
4
):
F301
F308
4
Walani
SR
.
Global burden of preterm birth
.
Int J Gynaecol Obstet
.
2020
;
150
(
1
):
31
33
5
Quigley
MA
,
Poulsen
G
,
Boyle
E
, et al
.
Early term and late preterm birth are associated with poorer school performance at age 5 years: a cohort study
.
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed
.
2012
;
97
(
3
):
F167
F173
6
Vohr
B
.
Long-term outcomes of moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term infants
.
Clin Perinatol
.
2013
;
40
(
4
):
739
751
7
Van Naarden Braun
K
,
Christensen
D
,
Schieve
L
,
Goodman
A
,
Yeargin-Allson
M
.
Has the prevalence of cerebral palsy changed over time? Trends by birth weight, gestational age, and race/ethnicity in a United States population-based study, 1985–2002
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2015
;
57
:
20
20
8
Allotey
J
,
Zamora
J
,
Cheong-See
F
, et al
.
Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children
.
BJOG
.
2018
;
125
(
1
):
16
25
9
Bhutta
AT
,
Cleves
MA
,
Casey
PH
,
Cradock
MM
,
Anand
KJ
.
Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis
.
JAMA
.
2002
;
288
(
6
):
728
737
10
Nielsen
TM
,
Pedersen
MV
,
Milidou
I
,
Glavind
J
,
Henriksen
TB
.
Long-term cognition and behavior in children born at early term gestation: a systematic review
.
Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand
.
2019
;
98
(
10
):
1227
1234
11
Osterman
M
,
Hamilton
BE
,
Martin
JA
,
Driscoll
AK
,
Valenzuela
CP
.
Births: Final Data For 2020
.
National Center for Health Statistics
;
2022
12
Jarjour
IT
.
Neurodevelopmental outcome after extreme prematurity: a review of the literature
.
Pediatr Neurol
.
2015
;
52
(
2
):
143
152
13
McIntyre
S
,
Morgan
C
,
Walker
K
,
Novak
I
.
Cerebral palsy--don’t delay
.
Dev Disabil Res Rev
.
2011
;
17
(
2
):
114
129
14
Novak
I
,
Hines
M
,
Goldsmith
S
,
Barclay
R
.
Clinical prognostic messages from a systematic review on cerebral palsy
.
Pediatrics
.
2012
;
130
(
5
):
e1285
e1312
15
Baird
G
,
McConachie
H
,
Scrutton
D
.
Parents’ perceptions of disclosure of the diagnosis of cerebral palsy
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2000
;
83
(
6
):
475
480
16
Moher
D
,
Shamseer
L
,
Clarke
M
, et al
;
PRISMA-P Group
.
Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement
.
Syst Rev
.
2015
;
4
(
1
):
1
17
Lawn
JE
,
Davidge
R
,
Paul
VK
, et al
.
Born too soon: care for the preterm baby
.
Reprod Health
.
2013
;
10 Suppl 1
(
Suppl 1
):
S5
18
NICE
.
Developmental Follow-up of Children and Young People Born Preterm NICE Guideline
.
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence
;
2017
19
Chan
E
,
Leong
P
,
Malouf
R
,
Quigley
MA
.
Long-term cognitive and school outcomes of late-preterm and early-term births: a systematic review
.
Child Care Health Dev
.
2016
;
42
(
3
):
297
312
20
Covidence
. Covidence. Available at: https://www.covidence.org/. Accessed January 25, 2023
21
Watson
PF
,
Petrie
A
.
Method agreement analysis: a review of correct methodology
.
Theriogenology
.
2010
;
73
(
9
):
1167
1179
22
Wells
GA
,
Shea
B
,
O’Connell
D
, et al
. The Newcastle-Ottawa scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomised studies in meta-analyses. Available from: www.ohri.ca/programs/clinical_epidemiology/oxford.asp. Accessed January 23, 2023
23
Gøtzsche
PC
,
Hróbjartsson
A
,
Maric
K
,
Tendal
B
.
Data extraction errors in meta-analyses that use standardized mean differences
.
JAMA
.
2007
;
298
(
4
):
430
437
24
Nyaga
VN
,
Arbyn
M
,
Aerts
M
.
Metaprop: a Stata command to perform meta-analysis of binomial data
.
Arch Public Health
.
2014
;
72
(
1
):
39
25
Barendregt
JJ
,
Doi
SA
,
Lee
YY
,
Norman
RE
,
Vos
T
.
Meta-analysis of prevalence
.
J Epidemiol Community Health
.
2013
;
67
(
11
):
974
978
26
Boyle
EM
,
Johnson
S
,
Manktelow
B
, et al
.
Neonatal outcomes and delivery of care for infants born late preterm or moderately preterm: a prospective population-based study
.
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed
.
2015
;
100
(
6
):
F479
F485
27
Quinn
J-A
,
Munoz
FM
,
Gonik
B
, et al
;
Brighton Collaboration Preterm Birth Working Group
.
Preterm birth: case definition & guidelines for data collection, analysis, and presentation of immunisation safety data
.
Vaccine
.
2016
;
34
(
49
):
6047
6056
28
Cheong
JL
,
Doyle
LW
,
Burnett
AC
, et al
.
Association between moderate and late preterm birth and neurodevelopment and social-emotional development at age 2 years
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2017
;
171
(
4
):
e164805
29
Patel
RM
.
Short- and long-term outcomes for extremely preterm infants
.
Am J Perinatol
.
2016
;
33
(
3
):
318
328
30
Schmidt
B
,
Anderson
PJ
,
Doyle
LW
, et al
;
Caffeine for Apnea of Prematurity (CAP) Trial Investigators
.
Survival without disability to age 5 years after neonatal caffeine therapy for apnea of prematurity
.
JAMA
.
2012
;
307
(
3
):
275
282
31
Blencowe
H
,
Lee
AC
,
Cousens
S
, et al
.
Preterm birth-associated neurodevelopmental impairment estimates at regional and global levels for 2010
.
Pediatr Res
.
2013
;
74 Suppl 1
(
Suppl 1
):
17
34
32
Woythaler
M
.
Neurodevelopmental outcomes of the late preterm infant
.
Semin Fetal Neonatal Med
.
2019
;
24
(
1
):
54
59
33
Kugelman
A
,
Colin
AA
.
Late preterm infants: near term but still in a critical developmental time period
.
Pediatrics
.
2013
;
132
(
4
):
741
751
34
Luu
TM
,
Rehman Mian
MO
,
Nuyt
AM
.
Long-term impact of preterm birth: neurodevelopmental and physical health outcomes
.
Clin Perinatol
.
2017
;
44
(
2
):
305
314
35
Kinney
HC
.
The near-term (late preterm) human brain and risk for periventricular leukomalacia: a review
.
Semin Perinatol
.
2006
;
30
(
2
):
81
88
36
Odd
DE
,
Emond
A
,
Whitelaw
A
.
Long-term cognitive outcomes of infants born moderately and late preterm
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2012
;
54
(
8
):
704
709
37
Kapellou
O
,
Counsell
SJ
,
Kennea
N
, et al
.
Abnormal cortical development after premature birth shown by altered allometric scaling of brain growth
.
PLoS Med
.
2006
;
3
(
8
):
e265
38
Picone
S
,
Aufieri
R
,
Paolillo
P
.
Infection in late preterm infants
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2014
;
90
(
Suppl 1
):
S71
S74
39
Baron
IS
,
Litman
FR
,
Ahronovich
MD
,
Baker
R
.
Late preterm birth: a review of medical and neuropsychological childhood outcomes
.
Neuropsychol Rev
.
2012
;
22
(
4
):
438
450
40
Brandon
DH
,
Tully
KP
,
Silva
SG
, et al
.
Emotional responses of mothers of late-preterm and term infants
.
J Obstet Gynecol Neonatal Nurs
.
2011
;
40
(
6
):
719
731
41
Lefkowitz
DS
,
Baxt
C
,
Evans
JR
.
Prevalence and correlates of posttraumatic stress and postpartum depression in parents of infants in the neonatal intensive care unit (NICU)
.
J Clin Psychol Med Settings
.
2010
;
17
(
3
):
230
237
42
Johnson
MH
.
Executive function and developmental disorders: the flip side of the coin
.
Trends Cogn Sci
.
2012
;
16
(
9
):
454
457
43
Diamond
A
.
Executive functions
.
Annu Rev Psychol
.
2013
;
64
:
135
168
44
Cairney
DG
,
Kazmi
A
,
Delahunty
L
,
Marryat
L
,
Wood
R
.
The predictive value of universal preschool developmental assessment in identifying children with later educational difficulties: a systematic review
.
PLoS One
.
2021
;
16
(
3
):
e0247299
45
Zhou
H
,
Qu
X
,
Yang
Y
, et al
.
Relationship between moderate to late preterm, diet types and developmental delay in less-developed rural China
.
Nutr Neurosci
.
2022
;
25
(
1
):
70
79
46
Hanly
M
,
Falster
K
,
Chambers
G
, et al
.
Gestational age and child development at age five in a population-based cohort of Australian aboriginal and non-aboriginal children
.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol
.
2018
;
32
(
1
):
114
125
47
Wang
CJ
,
McGlynn
EA
,
Brook
RH
, et al
.
Quality-of-care indicators for the neurodevelopmental follow-up of very low birth weight children: results of an expert panel process
.
Pediatrics
.
2006
;
117
(
6
):
2080
2092
48
Petrou
S
,
Abangma
G
,
Johnson
S
,
Wolke
D
,
Marlow
N
.
Costs and health utilities associated with extremely preterm birth: evidence from the EPICure study
.
Value Health
.
2009
;
12
(
8
):
1124
1134
49
Johnson
S
,
Gilmore
C
,
Gallimore
I
,
Jaekel
J
,
Wolke
D
.
The long-term consequences of preterm birth: what do teachers know?
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2015
;
57
(
6
):
571
577
50
Alderson
P
,
Hawthorne
J
,
Killen
M
.
Parents’ experiences of sharing neonatal information and decisions: consent, cost and risk
.
Soc Sci Med
.
2006
;
62
(
6
):
1319
1329
51
Hadders-Algra
M
.
Early diagnosis and early intervention in cerebral palsy
.
Front Neurol
.
2014
;
5
:
185
52
Spittle
A
,
Orton
J
,
Anderson
P
,
Boyd
R
,
Doyle
LW
.
Early developmental intervention programmes post-hospital discharge to prevent motor and cognitive impairments in preterm infants
.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev
.
2012
;
12
:
CD005495
53
Baron
IS
,
Erickson
K
,
Ahronovich
MD
,
Coulehan
K
,
Baker
R
,
Litman
FR
.
Visuospatial and verbal fluency relative deficits in ‘complicated’ late-preterm preschool children
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2009
;
85
(
12
):
751
754
54
Morse
SB
,
Zheng
H
,
Tang
Y
,
Roth
J
.
Early school-age outcomes of late preterm infants
.
Pediatrics
.
2009
;
123
(
4
):
e622
e629
55
Petrini
JR
,
Dias
T
,
McCormick Marie
C
,
Massolo
ML
,
Green
NS
,
Escobar
GJ
.
Increased risk of adverse neurological development for late preterm infants
.
J Pediatr
.
2009
;
154
(
2
):
169
176
56
Baron
IS
,
Erickson
K
,
Ahronovich
MD
,
Baker
R
,
Litman
FR
.
Cognitive deficit in preschoolers born late-preterm
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2011
;
87
(
2
):
115
119
57
Woythaler
MA
,
McCormick
MC
,
Smith
VC
.
Late preterm infants have worse 24-month neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants
.
Pediatrics
.
2011
;
127
(
3
):
e622
e629
58
Baron
IS
,
Kerns
KA
,
Müller
U
,
Ahronovich
MD
,
Litman
FR
.
Executive functions in extremely low birth weight and late-preterm preschoolers: effects on working memory and response inhibition
.
Child Neuropsychol
.
2012
;
18
(
6
):
586
599
59
Curry
AE
,
Pfeiffer
MR
,
Slopen
ME
,
McVeigh
KH
.
Rates of early intervention referral and significant developmental delay, by birthweight and gestational age
.
Matern Child Health J
.
2012
;
16
(
5
):
989
996
60
Talge
NM
,
Holzman
C
,
Van Egeren
LA
, et al
;
Late-Preterm Birth by Delivery Circumstance and Its Association With Parent-Reported Attention Problems in Childhood
.
Late-preterm birth by delivery circumstance and its association with parent-reported attention problems in childhood
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2012
;
33
(
5
):
405
415
61
Baron
IS
,
Weiss
BA
,
Baker
R
, et al
.
Subtle adverse effects of late preterm birth: a cautionary note
.
Neuropsychology
.
2014
;
28
(
1
):
11
18
62
Brumbaugh
JE
,
Hodel
AS
,
Thomas
KM
.
The impact of late preterm birth on executive function at preschool age
.
Am J Perinatol
.
2014
;
31
(
4
):
305
314
63
Kuzniewicz
MW
,
Wi
S
,
Qian
Y
,
Walsh
EM
,
Armstrong
MA
,
Croen
LA
.
Prevalence and neonatal factors associated with autism spectrum disorders in preterm infants
.
J Pediatr
.
2014
;
164
(
1
):
20
25
64
Richards
JL
,
Chapple-McGruder
T
,
Williams
BL
,
Kramer
MR
.
Does neighborhood deprivation modify the effect of preterm birth on children’s first grade academic performance?
Soc Sci Med
.
2015
;
132
:
122
131
65
Woythaler
M
,
McCormick
MC
,
Mao
WY
,
Smith
VC
.
Late preterm infants and neurodevelopmental outcomes at kindergarten
.
Pediatrics
.
2015
;
136
(
3
):
424
431
66
Hodel
AS
,
Brumbaugh
JE
,
Morris
AR
,
Thomas
KM
.
Hot executive function following moderate-to-late preterm birth: altered delay discounting at 4 years of age
.
Dev Sci
.
2016
;
19
(
2
):
221
234
67
Crockett
LK
,
Ruth
CA
,
Heaman
MI
,
Brownell
MD
.
Education outcomes of children born late preterm: a retrospective whole-population cohort study
.
Matern Child Health J
.
2022
;
26
(
5
):
1126
1141
68
Lingasubramanian
G
,
Corman
H
,
Noonan
K
,
Reichman
NE
.
Gestational age at term and teacher-reported attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptom patterns
.
J Pediatr
.
2022
;
251
:
120
126.e4
69
Wehby
GL
.
Gestational age, newborn metabolic markers and academic achievement
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2022
;
19
(
3
):
1549
70
Robaei
D
,
Kifley
A
,
Gole
GA
,
Mitchell
P
.
The impact of modest prematurity on visual function at age 6 years: findings from a population-based study
.
Arch Ophthalmol
.
2006
;
124
(
6
):
871
877
71
Raynes-Greenow
CH
,
Hadfield
RM
,
Cistulli
PA
,
Bowen
J
,
Allen
H
,
Roberts
CL
.
Sleep apnea in early childhood associated with preterm birth but not small for gestational age: a population-based record linkage study
.
Sleep
.
2012
;
35
(
11
):
1475
1480
72
Schneider
LA
,
Burns
NR
,
Giles
LC
, et al
.
Cognitive abilities in preterm and term-born adolescents
.
J Pediatr
.
2014
;
165
(
1
):
170
177
73
Smithers
LG
,
Searle
AK
,
Chittleborough
CR
,
Scheil
W
,
Brinkman
SA
,
Lynch
JW
.
A whole-of-population study of term and post-term gestational age at birth and children’s development
.
BJOG
.
2015
;
122
(
10
):
1303
1311
74
Searle
AK
,
Smithers
LG
,
Chittleborough
CR
,
Gregory
TA
,
Lynch
JW
.
Gestational age and school achievement: a population study
.
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed
.
2017
;
102
(
5
):
F409
F416
75
Brown
S
,
Quach
J
,
Anderson
PJ
,
Mensah
F
,
Roberts
G
.
Influence of gestational age and working memory on math skills in children aged 8 to 9 years
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2019
;
40
(
1
):
49
53
76
Dhamrait
GK
,
Christian
H
,
O’Donnell
M
,
Pereira
G
.
Gestational age and child development at school entry
.
Sci Rep
.
2021
;
11
(
1
):
14522
77
Poulsen
G
,
Wolke
D
,
Kurinczuk
JJ
, et al
.
Gestational age and cognitive ability in early childhood: a population-based cohort study
.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol
.
2013
;
27
(
4
):
371
379
78
Guy
A
,
Seaton
SE
,
Boyle
EM
, et al
.
Infants born late/moderately preterm are at increased
risk for a positive autism screen at 2 years of age
.
J Pediatr
.
2015
;
166
(
2
):
269
75.e3
79
Johnson
S
,
Matthews
R
,
Draper
ES
, et al
.
Early emergence of delayed social competence in infants born late and moderately preterm
.
J Dev Behav Pediatr
.
2015
;
36
(
9
):
690
699
80
Cronin
FM
,
Segurado
R
,
McAuliffe
FM
,
Kelleher
CC
,
Tremblay
RE
.
Gestational age at birth and ‘body-mind’ health at 5 years of age: a population based cohort study
.
PLoS One
.
2016
;
11
(
3
):
e0151222
81
Pettinger
KJ
,
Kelly
B
,
Sheldon
TA
,
Mon-Williams
M
,
Wright
J
,
Hill
LJB
.
Starting school: educational development as a function of age of entry and prematurity
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2020
;
105
(
2
):
160
165
82
Alterman
N
,
Johnson
S
,
Carson
C
, et al
.
Gestational age at birth and child special educational needs: a UK representative birth cohort study
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2021
;
106
(
9
):
842
848
83
Alterman
N
,
Johnson
S
,
Carson
C
, et al
.
Gestational age at birth and academic attainment in primary and secondary school in England: evidence from a national cohort study
.
PLoS One
.
2022
;
17
(
8
):
e0271952
84
Libuy
N
,
Gilbert
R
,
Mc Grath-Lone
L
,
Blackburn
R
,
Etoori
D
,
Harron
K
.
Gestational age at birth, chronic conditions and school outcomes: a population-based data linkage study of children born in England
.
Int J Epidemiol
.
2023
;
52
(
1
):
132
143
85
van Baar
AL
,
Vermaas
J
,
Knots
E
,
de Kleine
MJ
,
Soons
P
.
Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks’ gestational age
.
Pediatrics
.
2009
;
124
(
1
):
251
257
86
Bul
KC
,
van Baar
AL
.
Behavior problems in relation to sustained selective attention skills of moderately preterm children
.
J Dev Phys Disabil
.
2012
;
24
(
2
):
111
123
87
Cserjesi
R
,
Van Braeckel
KN
,
Butcher
PR
, et al
.
Functioning of 7-year-old children born at 32 to 35 weeks’ gestational age
.
Pediatrics
.
2012
;
130
(
4
):
e838
e846
88
de Jong
M
,
Verhoeven
M
,
Lasham
CA
,
Meijssen
CB
,
van Baar
AL
.
Behaviour and development in 24-month-old moderately preterm toddlers
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2015
;
100
(
6
):
548
553
89
Potijk
MR
,
de Winter
AF
,
Bos
AF
,
Kerstjens
JM
,
Reijneveld
SA
.
Behavioural and emotional problems in moderately preterm children with low socioeconomic status: a population-based study
.
Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry
.
2015
;
24
(
7
):
787
795
90
Hornman
J
,
de Winter
AF
,
Kerstjens
JM
,
Bos
AF
,
Reijneveld
SA
.
Stability of developmental problems after school entry of moderately-late preterm and early preterm-born children
.
J Pediatr
.
2017
;
187
:
73
79
91
Hua
J
,
Barnett
AL
,
Williams
GJ
, et al
.
Association of gestational age at birth with subsequent suspected developmental coordination disorder in early childhood in China
.
JAMA Netw Open
.
2021
;
4
(
12
):
e2137581
92
You
J
,
Shamsi
BH
,
Hao
MC
,
Cao
CH
,
Yang
WY
.
A study on the neurodevelopment outcomes of late preterm infants
.
BMC Neurol
.
2019
;
19
(
1
):
108
93
You
J
,
Yang
HJ
,
Hao
MC
,
Zheng
JJ
.
Late preterm infants’ social competence, motor development, and cognition
.
Front Psychiatry
.
2019
;
10
:
69
94
Stene-Larsen
K
,
Brandlistuen
RE
,
Lang
AM
,
Landolt
MA
,
Latal
B
,
Vollrath
ME
.
Communication impairments in early term and late preterm children: a prospective cohort study following children to age 36 months
.
J Pediatr
.
2014
;
165
(
6
):
1123
1128
95
Strand
KM
,
Heimstad
R
,
Iversen
AC
, et al
.
Mediators of the association between pre-eclampsia and cerebral palsy: population based cohort study
.
BMJ
.
2013
;
347
:
f4089
96
Ask
H
,
Gustavson
K
,
Ystrom
E
, et al
.
Association of gestational age at birth with symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children
.
JAMA Pediatr
.
2018
;
172
(
8
):
749
756
97
Zambrana
IM
,
Vollrath
ME
,
Jacobsson
B
,
Sengpiel
V
,
Ystrom
E
.
Preterm birth and risk for language delays before school entry: a sibling-control study
.
Dev Psychopathol
.
2021
;
33
(
1
):
47
52
98
Demestre
X
,
Schonhaut
L
,
Morillas
J
, et al
.
[Development deficit risks in the late premature newborn: evaluation at 48 months using the Ages & Stages Questionnaires®]
.
An Pediatr (Barc)
.
2016
;
84
(
1
):
39
45
99
Oros
D
,
Altermir
I
,
Elia
N
, et al
.
Pathways of neuronal and cognitive development in children born small-for-gestational age or late preterm
.
Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol
.
2014
;
43
(
1
):
41
47
100
Pérez-Pereira
M
,
Fernández
P
,
Gómez-Taibo
ML
,
Resches
M
.
Language development of low risk preterm infants up to the age of 30 months
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2014
;
90
(
10
):
649
656
101
Pérez-Pereira
M
,
Fernández
MP
,
Gómez-Taibo
ML
,
Martínez-López
Z
,
Arce
C
.
A follow-up study of cognitive development in low risk preterm children
.
Int J Environ Res Public Health
.
2020
;
17
(
7
):
2380
102
Klassen
AF
,
Lee
SK
,
Raina
P
,
Chan
HW
,
Matthew
D
,
Brabyn
D
.
Health status and health-related quality of life in a population-based sample of neonatal intensive care unit graduates
.
Pediatrics
.
2004
;
113
(
3 Pt 1
):
594
600
103
Leavey
A
,
Zwaigenbaum
L
,
Heavner
K
,
Burstyn
I
.
Gestational age at birth and risk of autism spectrum disorders in Alberta, Canada
.
J Pediatr
.
2013
;
162
(
2
):
361
368
104
Faleschini
S
,
Matte-Gagné
C
,
Côté
S
,
Tremblay
RE
,
Boivin
M
.
Trajectories of behavioral problems among moderate-late preterm children from 4 to 10 years: a prospective population-based study
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2020
;
143
:
104964
105
Chen
R
,
Tedroff
K
,
Villamor
E
,
Lu
D
,
Cnattingius
S
.
Risk of intellectual disability in children born appropriate-for-gestational-age at term or post-term: impact of birth weight for gestational age and gestational age
.
Eur J Epidemiol
.
2020
;
35
(
3
):
273
282
106
Beer
RJ
,
Cnattingius
S
,
Susser
ES
,
Villamor
E
.
Associations of preterm birth, small-for-gestational age, preeclampsia and placental abruption with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the offspring: nationwide cohort and sibling-controlled studies
.
Acta Paediatr
.
2022
;
111
(
8
):
1546
1555
107
Chen
R
,
Sjölander
A
,
Johansson
S
, et al
.
Impact of gestational age on risk of cerebral palsy: unravelling the role of neonatal morbidity
.
Int J Epidemiol
.
2022
;
50
(
6
):
1852
1863
108
Larsen
ML
,
Wiingreen
R
,
Jensen
A
, et al
.
The effect of gestational age on major neurodevelopmental disorders in preterm infants
.
Pediatr Res
.
2022
;
91
(
7
):
1906
1912
109
Zhu
JL
,
Olsen
J
,
Olesen
AW
.
Risk for developmental coordination disorder correlates with gestational age at birth
.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol
.
2012
;
26
(
6
):
572
577
110
Larroque
B
,
Ancel
PY
,
Marret
S
, et al
;
EPIPAGE Study group
.
Neurodevelopmental disabilities and special care of 5-year-old children born before 33 weeks of gestation (the EPIPAGE study): a longitudinal cohort study
.
Lancet
.
2008
;
371
(
9615
):
813
820
111
Bailhache
M
,
Monnier
M
,
Moulin
F
, et al
;
SAPRIS study group
.
Emotional and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms of preterm vs. full-term children during COVID-19 pandemic restrictions
.
Pediatr Res
.
2022
;
92
(
6
):
1749
1756
112
Voigt
B
,
Pietz
J
,
Pauen
S
,
Kliegel
M
,
Reuner
G
.
Cognitive development in very vs. moderately to late preterm and full-term children: can effortful control account for group differences in toddlerhood?
Early Hum Dev
.
2012
;
88
(
5
):
307
313
113
Reuner
G
,
Weinschenk
A
,
Pauen
S
,
Pietz
J
.
Cognitive development in 7- to 24-month-old extremely/very-to-moderately/late preterm and full-term born infants: the mediating role of focused attention
.
Child Neuropsychol
.
2015
;
21
(
3
):
314
330
114
Darlow
BA
,
Horwood
LJ
,
Wynn-Williams
MB
,
Mogridge
N
,
Austin
NC
.
Admissions of all gestations to a regional neonatal unit versus controls: 2-year outcome
.
J Paediatr Child Health
.
2009
;
45
(
4
):
187
193
115
Berry
MJ
,
Foster
T
,
Rowe
K
,
Robertson
O
,
Robson
B
,
Pierse
N
.
Gestational age, health, and educational outcomes in adolescents
.
Pediatrics
.
2018
;
142
(
5
):
e20181016
116
Yang
S
,
Platt
RW
,
Kramer
MS
.
Variation in child cognitive ability by week of gestation among healthy term births
.
Am J Epidemiol
.
2010
;
171
(
4
):
399
406
117
Polić
B
,
Bubić
A
,
Meštrović
J
, et al
.
Emotional and behavioral outcomes and quality of life in school-age children born as late preterm: retrospective cohort study
.
Croat Med J
.
2017
;
58
(
5
):
332
341
118
Hirvonen
M
,
Ojala
R
,
Korhonen
P
, et al
.
Cerebral palsy among children born moderately and late preterm
.
Pediatrics
.
2014
;
134
(
6
):
e1584
e1593
119
Drougia
A
,
Giapros
V
,
Krallis
N
, et al
.
Incidence and risk factors for cerebral palsy in infants with perinatal problems: a 15-year review
.
Early Hum Dev
.
2007
;
83
(
8
):
541
547
120
Tso
WW
,
Ho
FKW
,
Coghill
D
, et al
.
Preterm postnatal complications and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2022
;
65
(
3
):
358
366
121
Patil
YJ
,
Metgud
D
.
Comparison of non verbal learning difficulties in preschoolers born preterm with the term born peers
.
Indian J Pediatr
.
2014
;
81
(
4
):
346
349
122
Marotta
A
,
Rea
M
,
Ferri
R
, et al
.
Investigating socio-emotional cognition in late preterm children: a case-control study
.
Eur J Dev Psychol
.
2020
;
17
(
3
):
365
378
123
Higa Diez
M
,
Yorifuji
T
,
Kado
Y
,
Sanada
S
,
Doi
H
.
Preterm birth and behavioural outcomes at 8 years of age: a nationwide survey in Japan
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2016
;
101
(
4
):
338
343
124
Al-Haddad
C
,
Fattah
MA
,
Smeets
L
, et al
.
Ophthalmologic outcomes of children born premature without ROP: correlations with gestational age and psychomotor development
.
J Pediatr Ophthalmol Strabismus
.
2017
;
54
(
1
):
32
38
125
National Guideline Alliance (UK)
.
Mental Health Problems in People with Learning Disabilities: Prevention, Assessment and Management
.
National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE)
;
2016
126
National Institute for Heath and Care Excellence
. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis and management NICE guideline [NG87]. Available at: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng87/documents/search-strategies. Accessed February 7, 2023
127
Scantlebury
A
,
Mcdaid
C
,
Dawson
V
, et al
.
Non-pharmacological interventions for non-respiratory sleep disturbance in children with neurodisabilities: a systematic review
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2018
;
60
(
11
):
1076
1092
128
Mpundu-Kaambwa
C
,
Chen
G
,
Huynh
E
,
Russo
R
,
Ratcliffe
J
.
Protocol for a systematic review of instruments for the assessment of quality of life and well-being in children and adolescents with cerebral palsy
.
BMJ Open
.
2017
;
7
(
9
):
e015924
129
Canadian Health Libraries Association
. Recherche qualitative. Available at: https://extranet.santecom.qc.ca/wiki/!biblio3s/doku.php?id=concepts:recherche-qualitative. Accessed February 7, 2023
130
Canadian Health Libraries Association
. Lignes directrices. Available at: https://extranet.santecom.qc.ca/wiki/!biblio3s/doku.php?id=concepts:lignes-directrices. Accessed February 7, 2023
131
Canadian Health Libraries Association
. Revues de la littérature et méta-analyses. Available at: santecom.qc.ca. Accessed February 7, 2023
132
American University of Beirut
. Systematic reviews: health sciences. Available at: https://aub.edu.lb.libguides.com/c.php?g=329862&p=3023731. Accessed February 7, 2023
133
Wolters Kluwer Health
. Expert searches. Available at: https://tools.ovid.com/ovidtools/expertsearches.html. Accessed February 7, 2023
134
Nepomnyaschy
L
,
Hegyi
T
,
Ostfeld
BM
,
Reichman
NE
.
Developmental outcomes of late-preterm infants at 2 and 4 years
.
Matern Child Health J
.
2012
;
16
(
8
):
1612
1624
135
Williams
BL
,
Dunlop
AL
,
Kramer
M
,
Dever
BV
,
Hogue
C
,
Jain
L
.
Perinatal origins of first-grade academic failure: role of prematurity and maternal factors
.
Pediatrics
.
2013
;
131
(
4
):
693
700
136
Richards
JL
,
Drews-Botsch
C
,
Sales
JM
,
Flanders
WD
,
Kramer
MR
.
Describing the shape of the relationship between gestational age at birth and cognitive development in a nationally representative U.S. birth cohort
.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol
.
2016
;
30
(
6
):
571
582
137
Shah
P
,
Kaciroti
N
,
Richards
B
,
Oh
W
,
Lumeng
JC
.
Developmental outcomes of late preterm infants from infancy to kindergarten
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
2
):
e20153496
138
Shah
PE
,
Kaciroti
N
,
Richards
B
,
Lumeng
JC
.
Gestational age and kindergarten school readiness in a national sample of preterm infants
.
J Pediatr
.
2016
;
178
:
61
67
139
Beauregard
JL
,
Drews-Botsch
C
,
Sales
JM
,
Flanders
WD
,
Kramer
MR
.
Does socioeconomic status modify the association between preterm birth and children’s early cognitive ability and kindergarten academic achievement in the United States?
Am J Epidemiol
.
2018
;
187
(
8
):
1704
1713
140
Kerstjens
JM
,
de Winter
AF
,
Bocca-Tjeertes
IF
,
ten Vergert
EM
,
Reijneveld
SA
,
Bos
AF
.
Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children at school entry
.
J Pediatr
.
2011
;
159
(
1
):
92
98
141
Kerstjens
JM
,
de Winter
AF
,
Bocca-Tjeertes
IF
,
Bos
AF
,
Reijneveld
SA
.
Risk of developmental delay increases exponentially as gestational age of preterm infants decreases: a cohort study at age 4 years
.
Dev Med Child Neurol
.
2012
;
54
(
12
):
1096
1101
142
Potijk
MR
,
de Winter
AF
,
Bos
AF
,
Kerstjens
JM
,
Reijneveld
SA
.
Higher rates of behavioural and emotional problems at preschool age in children born moderately preterm
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2012
;
97
(
2
):
112
117
143
Potijk
MR
,
Kerstjens
JM
,
Bos
AF
,
Reijneveld
SA
,
de Winter
AF
.
Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children with low socioeconomic status: risks multiply
.
J Pediatr
.
2013
;
163
(
5
):
1289
1295
144
Hornman
J
,
de Winter
AF
,
Kerstjens
JM
,
Bos
AF
,
Reijneveld
SA
.
Emotional and behavioral problems of preterm and full-term children at school entry
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
137
(
5
):
e20152255
145
Potijk
MR
,
de Winter
AF
,
Bos
AF
,
Kerstjens
JM
,
Reijneveld
SA
.
Co-occurrence of developmental and behavioural problems in moderate to late preterm-born children
.
Arch Dis Child
.
2016
;
101
(
3
):
217
222
146
Chan
E
,
Quigley
MA
.
School performance at age 7 years in late preterm and early term birth: a cohort
study
.
Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed
.
2014
;
99
(
6
):
F451
F457
147
Fitzpatrick
A
,
Carter
J
,
Quigley
MA
.
Association of gestational age with verbal ability and spatial working memory at age 11
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
6
):
e20160578
148
Beauregard
JL
,
Drews-Botsch
C
,
Sales
JM
,
Flanders
WD
,
Kramer
MR
.
Preterm birth, poverty, and cognitive development
.
Pediatrics
.
2018
;
141
(
1
):
e20170509
149
Bentley
JP
,
Roberts
CL
,
Bowen
JR
,
Martin
AJ
,
Morris
JM
,
Nassar
N
.
Planned birth before 39 weeks and child development: a population-based study
.
Pediatrics
.
2016
;
138
(
6
):
e20162002
150
Arnaud
C
,
Daubisse-Marliac
L
,
White-Koning
M
, et al
.
Prevalence and associated factors of minor neuromotor dysfunctions at age 5 years in prematurely born children: the EPIPAGE Study
.
Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med
.
2007
;
161
(
11
):
1053
1061
This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits noncommercial, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Supplementary data